27.03.2013 Views

1 Neuroscience C 3045; Eye and Brain Professor M. Glickstein ...

1 Neuroscience C 3045; Eye and Brain Professor M. Glickstein ...

1 Neuroscience C 3045; Eye and Brain Professor M. Glickstein ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

<strong>Neuroscience</strong> C <strong>3045</strong>; <strong>Eye</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Brain</strong><br />

<strong>Professor</strong> M. <strong>Glickstein</strong><br />

January 2012<br />

The general problem of vision; what sorts of things can you do with your eyes? The<br />

eye, like other excellent optical devices forms an image of the world in front of it. Visual<br />

direction is probably innate. In addition there is vision for:<br />

Fine detail: Detection of a small object against a background (visual acuity). Clinical<br />

measures 20/20 [6/6] you see @ 20 feet what a normal eye sees at 20 feet. Acuity<br />

falls off in peripheral retina.<br />

Sensitivity: How much light do you need to see? Each rod can be activated by a single<br />

photon.<br />

Visual Depth <strong>and</strong> Distance Perception: How far away is an object? Which of two objects is<br />

closer?<br />

Monocular Mechanisms (e.g. Motion parallax; relative angular movement of 2 objects<br />

at different distances).<br />

Binocular Stereopsis Fusing of 2 disparate images to form a depth cue.<br />

Colour Vision: Identifying, naming, discriminating colours. How is colour processed in the<br />

brain?<br />

Form vision: Recognition of your friend's car in front of his house; Recognition of your<br />

friend's face.<br />

Movement Detection:<br />

Is an object stationary or moving?<br />

Visual guidance of your own movement.<br />

<strong>Eye</strong> movements (saccadic, pursuit; vergence).<br />

Accommodation<br />

Fingers <strong>and</strong> limbs (grasping an object in front of you)<br />

Whole body; walking along a crowded street: climbing a tree or the north fact of the<br />

Eiger.<br />

Related problem of distinguishing self-movement from movement of something in our<br />

field of view.<br />

The constancies: Why does brightness remain constant over a broad range of illumination<br />

intensities? Lump of coal in sunlight.<br />

When we consider the anatomy <strong>and</strong> physiology of the visual pathways, remember that it is<br />

these phenomena of vision that direct <strong>and</strong> focus research. How does the brain do it?<br />

1


Comparative anatomy of the vertebrate <strong>Eye</strong><br />

Discuss the classical light-microscopic view of the structure of the vertebrate eye with some<br />

general principles that apply to all vertebrates <strong>and</strong> examples of specialised structures that<br />

have evolved in one or another form.<br />

Similarity. To a great degree, all vertebrate eyes are similar. They share an almost identical<br />

imaging system (modified in special environments, such as under water).<br />

Compare with compound eye.<br />

Basic Cellular Plan The basic structural elements of the retina are similar in all vertebrates.<br />

Three nuclear layers with five or six cell-types involved in visual transduction<br />

<strong>and</strong> coding<br />

Two synaptic (plexiform) layers<br />

Evolution; Darwin’s difficulty<br />

“….if numerous gradations from a perfect <strong>and</strong> complex eye to one very<br />

imperfect <strong>and</strong> simple, each grade being useful to its possessor can be show to<br />

exist…then the difficulty… can hardly be considered real” See article by Lamb<br />

et.al. in reference list.<br />

Normal Human <strong>Eye</strong><br />

3 "Tunics" of eye<br />

A: Sclera<br />

B: Choroid <strong>and</strong> iris muscles<br />

C: Retina (<strong>and</strong> pigment epithelium)<br />

Note also focusing apparatus; cornea <strong>and</strong> lens <strong>and</strong> the optic nerve<br />

Differences. Where vertebrate eyes differ it is often related to the light environment in which<br />

the animal lives.<br />

Range of Light Intensity. Range of Physical Intensity approaches 10 12 . No sense organ<br />

could work effectively over such a range (10 3 /second action potentials).<br />

How to Code? Scale compression (Weber's law). We are sensitive to relative, not absolute<br />

light levels.<br />

How to deal with great range in light intensities<br />

1. Photomechanical Changes<br />

Physically restrict the amount of light which is allowed to reach receptors<br />

2. Parallel set of receptors (rods <strong>and</strong> cones) with different threshold <strong>and</strong> different<br />

dynamic range<br />

3. "Tuning" ganglion cells so that they report differences in light intensities within<br />

receptive field (lump of coal in sunlight looks black)<br />

2


Mechanical Regulation of Light at the Receptor<br />

Pupils<br />

Tiger<br />

What's wrong with the picture?<br />

Association of slits with nocturnal life style (esp. Snakes)<br />

Round vs. slit pupils. Nocturnal retina requires exclusion of light in daylight "basking"<br />

Pigments <strong>and</strong> stray light. Stray light degrades the image, it is absorbed in cameras <strong>and</strong> in the<br />

eye by pigment layers.<br />

Monkey<br />

Pigment epithelium <strong>and</strong> choroid<br />

Pigment epithelium plays an especially important role in light <strong>and</strong> dark adaptation of lower<br />

vertebrates. Processes interdigitate with tips of receptors.<br />

Pigment epithelium (Hedgehog)<br />

Pigment epithelium is a channel for (choroidal) vascular supply for receptors. Bites off<br />

growing rod tips.<br />

Pigment migration in lower vertebrate. The processes of pigment epithelial cells may extend<br />

the length of the receptors.<br />

Image 2 hairbrushes (receptors)<br />

Melanin granules are actively migrating:<br />

Migrate outward in light back (towards cell body) in dark<br />

Dark-adapted fish eye<br />

Light adapted fish eye<br />

Augmentation of Sensitivity. Animals that live in dim light-environment sacrifice precision<br />

of an image for increased sensitivity "snapshots vs. time exposures"<br />

Suppose light goes through receptor layer without ever being absorbed. If it strikes a<br />

reflective surface the receptors have a second chance<br />

<strong>Eye</strong> shine Leopard<br />

Tapetum Lucidum<br />

(Note Absence of melanin pigment in pigment epithelium)<br />

Toxicity of Excessive Light<br />

Albinos are especially at risk. Four weeks of continuous moderate illumination; albino rat<br />

causes degeneration of receptors<br />

Normal rat retina<br />

Constant light; retinal degeneration<br />

Retinal structure <strong>and</strong> the accuracy of ophthalmic instruments. To a reasonable approximation<br />

the retina of all mammals (vertebrates) is of constant thickness.<br />

Rat<br />

Whale<br />

Receptors <strong>and</strong> inner nuclear cells are roughly equivalent in size <strong>and</strong> number; related to the<br />

fact that these retinal elements do not conduct action potentials.<br />

Receptors <strong>and</strong> inner nuclear cells convey information over a short length by graded potentials<br />

(receptors, bipolar <strong>and</strong> horizontals some amacrine)<br />

Ganglion cell carries impulses to brain. The distance varies from a few millimetres to many<br />

centimetres. Ganglion cell sizes vary greatly. There is also variability within a single retina:<br />

Size of cell body is related to fibre diameter.<br />

Consequences of finite retinal thickness for ophthalmoscope <strong>and</strong> retinoscope. Shine light in;<br />

observe reflected shadow<br />

3


Suppose light is reflected from layer other than rods <strong>and</strong> cones. For thin lens in air<br />

D = f -- 1 by definition<br />

- 2<br />

dD = -f<br />

df or error (if constant) should be a function of the inverse square<br />

of the focal length<br />

Raw data; <strong>Eye</strong> size <strong>and</strong> refraction<br />

Log plot<br />

Conclude: retinoscope uses a specular reflection from vitreous-retinal surface<br />

Similar factor applies to opthalmoscope<br />

Growth <strong>and</strong> Plasticity of <strong>Eye</strong> Size; Emmetropization<br />

The normal newborn human infant eye measures 17 mm. from cornea to retina; the adult eye<br />

24 mm.<br />

The (strange but true ) evidence that eye growth is under local visual control.<br />

Different focal planes in the bird eye<br />

Fundamental Dichotomy of Rods <strong>and</strong> Cones<br />

Schultze's dichotomy: Differences in receptor types correlated with the life of the animal:<br />

"pure rod" <strong>and</strong> "pure cone" retinas.<br />

Retina is mixed rod/cone<br />

Monkey retina 3 layers<br />

Rod/cone (nuclei). Note Difference in nuclei of rods <strong>and</strong> cones.<br />

Higher power; conical outer segments<br />

Variability in cone density in human (an monkey) retina; rods are relatively constant<br />

Peripheral retina<br />

10 0 OUT<br />

Follow colour of nuclei<br />

Edge of fovea<br />

Central fovea<br />

Other Plans; The Retinal Streak<br />

Lindsay Johnson reindeer<br />

Note 1) Pigment distribution<br />

2) Vascular differences<br />

Receptors <strong>and</strong> Visual Pigments<br />

Wavelength sensitivity of Receptors.<br />

Outer segments of rods <strong>and</strong> cones contain photopigments.<br />

1) Absorb light 2) Transduce physical signal into an electrical one:<br />

Wavelength <strong>and</strong> Absorption<br />

Rods peak absorption at 500 nm<br />

2) Absorption spectra of pigment in cones. In human <strong>and</strong> monkey. 3 classes by<br />

microspectrophotometry; intracellular recording.<br />

Requirements for Colour Vision<br />

Colour vision requires at least 2 classes of receptor with different wavelength subdivisions.<br />

In principle you could do it either with pigments or with filters.<br />

Oil droplets<br />

Variability in Receptor Morphology Cones <strong>and</strong> Rods may be Thin or Fat<br />

Frog; Rods <strong>and</strong> cones<br />

4


Note massive rod (also "green" rods; double cones)<br />

Now cut parallel to the inner segments<br />

Retinal mosaic in Gecko.<br />

Gecko retina<br />

Synaptic connections of the retina<br />

Structure of the eye; Cajal 1892<br />

Cajal; Mammalian retina<br />

elements: receptors contact both horizontal <strong>and</strong> bipolar cells<br />

bipolar cells contact amacrine <strong>and</strong> ganglion cells<br />

amacrine cells contact bipolars <strong>and</strong> ganglion cells<br />

ganglion cells as final common path<br />

There are two synaptic layers in the eye. Outer plexiform layer containing cone pedicles, rod<br />

spherules; bipolar <strong>and</strong> horizontal cell processes<br />

Much more complex inner plexiform layer. Many cell types in the ganglion cell layer.<br />

Laminar structure of inner plexiform layer<br />

Ground squirrel<br />

Differences in ganglion cell size<br />

Cat retina<br />

Functional differences between ganglion cells of different morphology.<br />

1. Large somas have largest-calibre axons (m cells: transient properties)<br />

2. Smaller sized (p cells)<br />

On vs. off centre ganglion cells are associated with different planes within the inner<br />

plexiform layer<br />

The conduction pathway within the eye. Axons take a varied course, some sweeping away<br />

from the fovea enroute to optic nerve (myelinated only when they reach the disc hence non<br />

opaque)<br />

Note Blue Arcs in demonstrations<br />

Monkey whole mount<br />

Convergence & Divergence within the Retina<br />

Receptors as "points"<br />

Ganglion cells as transmitters<br />

Great variability in the complexity of inner nuclear layer<br />

Convergence is generally an adaptation for sensitivity<br />

Hamster<br />

But many animals (especially birds!) have massive inner nuclear layer: an active filter<br />

Tree shrew<br />

Humans <strong>and</strong> monkeys have highly convergent peripheral retina roughly equal number<br />

of receptors/inner nuclear cells/ganglion cells near fovea<br />

Monkey fofea<br />

Why is fovea shaped as it is?<br />

Owl fovea<br />

Chameleon (Anolis)<br />

5


Two aims;<br />

Central visual pathways <strong>and</strong> visuo-motor links<br />

`Introduce the targets of the eye, <strong>and</strong> particularly the primary visual cortex<br />

Discuss the connections from visual areas of the cortex to motor areas.<br />

I will talk about the connections from the eye, the nature of the crossing at the optic chiasma,<br />

<strong>and</strong> the primary <strong>and</strong> secondary targets of the optic tracts. I will introduce these topics in a<br />

historical context; how did we learn about the primary visual cortical area?<br />

I will review the evidence that led to our recognition of a primary motor area of the cerebral<br />

cortex, <strong>and</strong> early ideas about how the visual input might be linked to the motor output for<br />

visual guidance of the arm, the legs <strong>and</strong> the fingers. There are three major ways in which the<br />

visual areas might be linked to motor areas;<br />

1) Via a series of cortico-cortical links<br />

2) By way of basal ganglia<br />

3) A major pathway from cortex to the cerebellum by way of the pontine nuclei<br />

THE EYE AND THE CENTRAL VISUAL PATHWAYS<br />

. Much of the visual system can be thought of as in a series-like organization.<br />

Slide 1: Rhesus monkey retina ; low power to show series like arrangement of<br />

layers<br />

Receptors: (Rod/cones) Inner nuclear cells: (Horizontal, bipolar, amacrine) to ganglion<br />

cells.<br />

Visual information is imaged on the photoreceptors <strong>and</strong> must be relayed via inner-nuclear<br />

layer to ganglion cells for transmission to the brain.<br />

Centre-surround organization of retinal ganglion cells <strong>and</strong> scaling of relative intensity.<br />

Within this series-like arrangement, there are at every level, parallel processing mechanisms.<br />

Easiest to see at the level of the photoreceptors themselves.<br />

Rods <strong>and</strong> cones share the image plane: process visual information in parallel.<br />

Slide 2: High power to show rod <strong>and</strong> cone nuclei<br />

At the retinal ganglion-cell level. There are ganglion cells with grossly different structure,<br />

connections <strong>and</strong> function.<br />

Ratio of receptor to ganglion cells varies greatly<br />

Slide 3: Cat retina (note tepetum) Note differences in ganglion cell size<br />

Galgion cells axons project to brain<br />

Note large <strong>and</strong> smaller ganglion cells (m <strong>and</strong> p cells). Correlated with fibre-diameter.<br />

These different sized cells have different receptive field properties, different targets in the<br />

brain <strong>and</strong> different functions.<br />

6


Large cells (m cells); respond transiently to changes of illumination in their receptive field;<br />

<strong>and</strong> there are relatively more of them in peripheral retina.<br />

Smaller sized (p cells); respond in a sustained manner to appropriate illumination of their<br />

receptive field, have small receptive fields <strong>and</strong> are more heavily concentrated near fovea.<br />

Much smaller cells “k” cells (konio; dust) <strong>and</strong> light detection<br />

There is a smaller class of cells, the so-called “k” cells which have different receptive field<br />

properties <strong>and</strong> different structures. K cells only account for an estimated 10% of all retinal<br />

ganglion cells, but they seem to have important functions as light detectors.<br />

An interesting class of ganglion cell has been discovered relatively recently; the melanopsin<br />

containing ganglion cells. These cells may receive an input from photoreceptors, but they<br />

also have a photosensitive pigment within them. Melanopsin ganglion cells respond to a short<br />

wavelength of 484 nm peak sensitivity.<br />

“Tiling of retinal ganglion cells” <strong>and</strong> the extent of ganglion cell dendritic tree<br />

Slide 4: Cajal; Mammalian retina<br />

See reference to Dacey et. al. in reference list<br />

The three cell types cover the entire retina. Since there are about 80% p cells, 10% m cells<br />

<strong>and</strong> an estimated 10% k cells, it follows that if the entire retina is covered by the dendrites of<br />

these cells, the m <strong>and</strong> k cells must have larger dendritic territories than the p cell.<br />

Magnocellular/parvocellular division of LGN; Also interstitial cells<br />

Axons of the optic nerve<br />

Axons of the ganglion cell collect at the back of the eye after running along the inner retinal<br />

surface as unmyelinated fibres. (Why are they not yet myelinated?)<br />

Blind spot <strong>and</strong> exit zone.<br />

Slide 5: Monkey eye to show optic nerve (blue-arcs in demo)<br />

On the course of the optic nerve<br />

Optic nerve fibres proceed backwards <strong>and</strong> seem to unite in the X-shaped Optic Chiasma<br />

(Greek X = Chi). The re-sorted fibres after the chiasma are called the optic tract.<br />

At the chiasma the optic nerve fibres arising from ganglion cells in the nasal retina cross;<br />

those from the temporal retina remain uncrossed.<br />

The effect of this pattern of crossing is to project the right visual field onto the left<br />

hemisphere.<br />

Slide 6: To show location of chiasma at the ventral surface of the brain<br />

There was no agreement on what happens at the chiasma. Some believed in no<br />

decussation, some total decussation. (Isaac Newton had suggested the correct<br />

arrangement)<br />

There was always the problem of why we see singly with two eyes. Solved in different ways<br />

by different authors.<br />

Slide 7: Des Cartes; Chiasma <strong>and</strong> pineal<br />

Slide 8: Hemi decussation; John Taylor; Frankfort 1750<br />

Left Visual Field is projected onto the right side of the brain.<br />

On the consequences of lesions 1) before the chiasma 2) of the crossing fibres in the chiasma<br />

3) of the optic tract <strong>and</strong> beyond.<br />

HEMI-AN-OPIAS<br />

7


Homonymous (right or left)<br />

Heteronymous (bitemporal or binasal)<br />

Mnemonic: a person (with optic tract), visual radiations, or complete unilateral cortical<br />

lesion sees only on the side of the lesion.<br />

Partial decussation in dogs, cats <strong>and</strong> horses. How far lateral is the eye? Principal of Visual<br />

Field Representation in mammals.<br />

Slide 9: Tree shrew to show laterality of the eyes<br />

Slide 10: Tree Shrew; LGN <strong>and</strong> optic tract<br />

On the targets of the optic tract<br />

The targets in terms of relative numbers of fibres in man are:<br />

Lateral Geniculate Nucleus (LGN). The major target in humans<br />

Superior Colliculus<br />

Pretectal Nucleus; role in pupillary control<br />

Accessory optic system<br />

Hypothalamus particularly the suprachiasmatic nucles. Role in diurnal cycling.<br />

Lateral Geniculate Nucleus in turn relays visual information to the cortex.<br />

The Laminar Organization of the LGN.<br />

Slide 11: Human LGN (Recall centre surround receptive fields of LGN cells<br />

Strict segregation of input, but perefectly aligned. Problem of the blind spot.<br />

Phenomenon of transneuronal atrophy (Minkowski, 1920)<br />

Slide 12: LGN: Normal monkey<br />

Slide 13: Trans-neuronal atrophy<br />

Numbering from ventral to dorsal 1-6<br />

Contralateral eye projects to layers 1, 4 <strong>and</strong> 6<br />

Ipsilateral eye projects to layers 2, 3 <strong>and</strong> 5<br />

Note important distinction of<br />

Magnocellular layers (1 <strong>and</strong> 2) <strong>and</strong><br />

Parvocellular layers (3, 4, 5, <strong>and</strong> 6) laminae<br />

K cell projection to cells within the interlaminar fibre layers.<br />

Receptive fields of cells in the LGN<br />

Magnocellular layers relay m cells; achromatic; transient<br />

Parvocellular layers relay p cells colour-opponent; sustained<br />

K cells as light detectors; other properties<br />

Slide 14: Human colliculus; parasagittal<br />

The superior colliculus<br />

The superior colliculus receives a direct input from the optic tract <strong>and</strong> from the cerebral<br />

cortex.<br />

Importantly involved in the control of saccadic eye movements.<br />

Lesions of superior colliculus produce a transient deficit in initiating eye movements. Large<br />

lesion of the entire colliculus leads to a transient fixed gaze; which recovers in a few days.<br />

Lesions of Area 8 of the cerebral cortex (the "frontal eye fields") also produce a temporary<br />

deficit in initiating eye movements.<br />

Combined bilateral lesions of both lead to a permanent <strong>and</strong> severe deficit in initiating eye<br />

movements.<br />

8


Pretectal Nuclei <strong>and</strong> the pathway for the pupillary reflex<br />

Direct <strong>and</strong> consensual pupillary reflexes illumination of the eye causes pupillary constriction<br />

Retinal ganglion cells (certain small cells) Pretectal nuclei<br />

Edinger Westphal nucleus in midbrain <strong>and</strong> thence via NIII to Ciliary ganglion controlling<br />

Radial muscles of the iris.<br />

Melanopsin ganglion cells as another input to pupillary control.<br />

THE GENICULO-CORTICAL PROJECTION<br />

Slide 15: Brodmann, 1905; Monkey cortical areas, lateral<br />

Slide 16: Brodmann, 1905; Medial view<br />

Slide 17: Human Weil stain to show stripe of Gennari<br />

Segregation of the input from the left <strong>and</strong> right eye in the visual cortex; ocular dominance<br />

columns.<br />

We saw that projections from the left <strong>and</strong> right eyes are segregated in their connections to<br />

LGN. This segregation is maintained at the first cortical synapse (in lamina 4 of cortex). The<br />

segregation shows up in both histological <strong>and</strong> physiological experiments.<br />

Inject one eye of a monkey with a labelled amino acid. Taken up <strong>and</strong> transported to LGN.<br />

Some labelled protein taken up by neurons across the synapse <strong>and</strong> transported by LGN<br />

neurons to the cortex. Terminals of left & right eye run in parallel stripes of about 0.5 mm<br />

each on the cortex within layer IV.<br />

Slide 17: Ocular dominance columns; Hubel<br />

If one eye is masked in infancy vision is severely impaired in the deprived eye, the ocular<br />

dominance columns become asymmetric; <strong>and</strong> few cells can be activated by the deprived eye.<br />

"Take over" is failure to retract terminals.<br />

On the normal growth of the eye in development<br />

Slide 18: Human eye; neonate <strong>and</strong> adult<br />

Slide 19: Wiesel <strong>and</strong> Raviola ; Normal <strong>and</strong> form-deprived eye<br />

The form-deprived eyegrows much longer, <strong>and</strong> is seriously myopic<br />

Cells in more superficial <strong>and</strong> deep layers of cortex begin the process of fusing the input from<br />

the two eyes;<br />

Cytochrome oxidase "patches" or “blobs”in primary visual cortex<br />

Livingston <strong>and</strong> Hubel showed that colour information is targeted on the cytochrome oxidase<br />

patches. But nocturnal primates who lack colour vision also have the cytochrome oxidase<br />

patches.<br />

Multiple visual areas<br />

Slide 20: Brodmann; lateral view of monkey brain<br />

Slide 21: Brodmann; Medial view of monkey brain<br />

Is the striate cortex the sole target of LGN axons? Old evidence form the cat, recent<br />

evidence in monkeys. Macular sparing “blindsight” <strong>and</strong> other odds <strong>and</strong> ends<br />

9


VISUAL GUIDANCE OF MOVEMENT<br />

1. Some general problems: What do you want to move?<br />

Slide 1) YO YO MA PLAYING THE CELLO<br />

Slide 2) Bouree from Bach’s 3d suite for unaccompanied cello<br />

Demo Catching a small object<br />

Slide 3) (MONKEY REACHING FOR A PELLET<br />

Arms, legs etc. (catching a cricket ball/baseball)<br />

Fingers <strong>and</strong> wrist (threading a needle)<br />

<strong>Eye</strong>s: On the categories of voluntary eye movements conjugate vs vergence<br />

Smooth pursuit vs saccadic<br />

Why has so much attention been paid to eye movements?<br />

General problem of load compensation in limbs;<br />

<strong>Eye</strong>s always weigh the same<br />

How are visual areas connected to motor areas of the brain.<br />

Visual cortex to motor cortex; (superior colliculus to red nucleus).<br />

Three possible routes: 1) Cortico-cortical circuits. 2) Basal Ganglia, 3) Cerebellar<br />

Side look into history. The discovery of the motor cortex<br />

Slide 4: FRITSCH AND HITZIG<br />

Electrical stimulation<br />

Slide 5: MOTOR CORTEX<br />

The first attempt to identify the visual area led to an error.<br />

FERRIER: "CAUSING BLINDNESS"<br />

Ferrier's mistake was gently pointed out to him by Hermann Munk<br />

MUNK: OCCIPITAL LESIONS CAUSES HEMIANOPIA<br />

SALOMON HENSCHEN “HEMIANOPISCHE FÄLLE”<br />

“KEINE HEMIANOPSIE”<br />

By the end of the nineteenth century, the identity <strong>and</strong> location of the primary visual<br />

<strong>and</strong> motor areas of the human <strong>and</strong> monkey cerebral cortex were clear. Less was understood<br />

about further processing of the visual signal.<br />

BRODMANN LATERAL VIEW<br />

Note Area 18 <strong>and</strong> 19; "Association areas"<br />

BRODMANN MEDIAL VIEW<br />

Parietal Lobe Lesions Cause Visuo Motor Impairment<br />

Why did Ferrier make the mistakes that he did? In his earliest experiments, Ferrier operated<br />

in unsterile conditions. Because of the risk of infections, he killed the monkeys three days<br />

after they were operated on. He later joined the faculty of the Medical School at King's <strong>and</strong><br />

learned sterile operative technique from his colleague, Joseph Lister.<br />

His animals could now live for weeks, months or years. In the second edition of his book,<br />

Functions of the <strong>Brain</strong>, he modified his statement<br />

FERRIER: "TEMPORARY COMPLETE ...."<br />

Angular gyrus <strong>and</strong> parietal lobe<br />

BALINTS CASE<br />

BALINTS DESCRIPTION<br />

What is the Circuit between Visual cortex <strong>and</strong> Motor Cortex?<br />

10


BERNSTEIN 1900<br />

The discovery of the motor cortex. Fritsch & Hitzig's Experiment<br />

The discovery of the visual cortex. Ferrier's Mistake: Munk<br />

The "obvious" nature of cortico-cortical links.<br />

If cortico-cortical circuits are essential, the first link must be by way of the parietal lobe<br />

.<br />

Special problem of integration between the hemispheres (backh<strong>and</strong>ed shot in tennis).<br />

The evidence (or lack of it) for the role of cortico-cortical fibres.<br />

. Section of corpus callosum does not produce obvious deficits in bimanual<br />

coordination or sensory guidance of movement;<br />

Direct cuts of cortico-cortical fibres do not block visual guidance of the h<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong> arm.<br />

(Myers, Sperry & McCurdy)<br />

MYERS ET AL 1963<br />

But see Kuyper's interpretation<br />

.<br />

The Corticoponto-cerebellar pathway.<br />

Corticopontine (<strong>and</strong> tectopontine) fibres:<br />

HUMAN BASIS PEDUNCULI<br />

How to trap them<br />

Basis pedunculi - (pyramidal tract + corticocular fibres)<br />

Corticopontine fibres<br />

HUMAN MEDULLA<br />

PONS<br />

MATANO, STEPHAN<br />

How Does the cerebral Cortex Connect to the Pontine Nuclei?<br />

Two related questions;<br />

1) Which Cells<br />

2) Which Areas<br />

Inject pontine nuclei with a retrograde tracer; Identify labelled cortical cells<br />

TREE SHREW<br />

MONKEY: SOURCE OF CORTICO PONTINE VISUAL FIBRES<br />

VAN ESSEN EXTRA-STRIATE VISUAL AREAS<br />

NUMBER OF VISUAL AREAS AS A FUNCTION OF TIME<br />

Distiction of parietal <strong>and</strong> temporal lobe directed cortical connections<br />

1) Layer V pyramidal cells.<br />

2) The differential distribution from extrastriate visual areas in the monkey.<br />

(Superior Colliculus; Pretectum <strong>and</strong> LGN)<br />

11


Pontocerebellar Pathway<br />

Cerebellar afferent fibres<br />

Mossy vs. climbing fibres<br />

3 peduncles<br />

HUMAN PONS; SAGITTAL SECTION<br />

MIDDLE PEDUNCLE<br />

middle > (inferior <strong>and</strong> superior)<br />

all (well nearly all) from n. pontis.<br />

Contralateral <strong>and</strong> ipsilateral projection in the middle cerebellar peduncle: Possible solution to<br />

some old problems.<br />

INFERIOR OLIVE (HUMAN)<br />

Climbing fibre input to the cerebellum. (J. Simpson)<br />

Accessory optic system dorsal cap of the inferior olivary nucleus. Flocculus <strong>and</strong> adjacent<br />

flocculus.<br />

On the difference between mossy fibre <strong>and</strong> climbing fibre visual afferents to the cerebellum.<br />

Some behavioural evidence for cerebellar role in visuo motor coordination <strong>and</strong> plasticity <strong>and</strong><br />

some unresolved questions.<br />

1. Plasticity of the VOR<br />

2. Saccadic Adaptation<br />

2. Prism adaptation<br />

3. Nictitating membrane conditioning<br />

References<br />

<strong>Eye</strong> <strong>and</strong> Central Visual pathways<br />

Classic work on the comparative anatomy of the eye is:<br />

Walls,G. (1942) The Vertebrate <strong>Eye</strong> <strong>and</strong> its Adaptive radiation Bloomfield Hills; Cranbrook<br />

A good general source for sensory function<br />

Barlow, H., <strong>and</strong> Mollon, J. (1983) The senses Cambridge<br />

A recent massive reference :<br />

Chalupa, L., <strong>and</strong> Werner, J. (2003) The Visual <strong>Neuroscience</strong>s Cambridge, MIT Press<br />

9:<br />

Dacey DM, Packer OS. (2003), Colour coding in the primate retina: diverse cell types <strong>and</strong><br />

cone-specific circuitry. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2003 Aug;13(4):421-7. Review.<br />

Functions beginning to catch up with Cajal’s anatomy.<br />

Lamb et. A;.(2007), Evolution of the vertebrate eye: opsins , photoreceptors, retibna <strong>and</strong> eye<br />

cup. Nature <strong>Neuroscience</strong> Reviews; 8 . 960-975.<br />

Evidenvce of evolution beginning to reassure Darwin<br />

For the central visual pathways, any good neuroscience text.<br />

<strong>Eye</strong> Movements<br />

Robinson, D.A. (1981) Control of eye movements in (V. Brooks, Ed.). American<br />

Physiological Society H<strong>and</strong>book of Physiology Section 1. The Nervous System, Volume II<br />

Motor Cortical, Ch. 28, 1275-1320.<br />

Carpenter, R.H.S. (1988) Movements of the <strong>Eye</strong>s. London: Pion.<br />

Lee, R.J., <strong>and</strong> Zee, D.S. (1999) The Neurology of <strong>Eye</strong> Movements New York; Oxford U<br />

12


ENTOPTIC PHENOMENA, VISUAL PHENOMENA AND EYE ANATOMY<br />

The pupil as a first line of defence against excessive light.<br />

DEMONSTRATION 1. PUPIL SHAPE; SIZE CHANGE<br />

Make a triangular shape with the tips of your fingers; bring the triangle closer to your eye:<br />

What happens to the shape of the triangle?<br />

Now open <strong>and</strong> close the other eye. Observe your own (consensual) Pupillary Reflex.<br />

The pupillary reflex has been well studied quantitatively. If we arbitrarily increase gain of<br />

the pupillary reflex.<br />

DEMONSTRATION 2. OSCILLATING PUPIL<br />

Shine penlight just on the edge of the pupil. Pupil will oscillate in size.<br />

In general with poor focus as in uncorrected myopia size of blur circles is related to the size<br />

of pupil. Therefore decreasing pupil size causes improved resolution with sufficient ambient<br />

light.<br />

DEMONSTRATION 3. REFRACTIVE DISORDER/PINHOLE<br />

Chose a myopic subject: Find the far point of distinct vision by seeing how far they can see<br />

the tip of a pin clearly. Place text just beyond far point so as to be unreadable. Now view<br />

through narrow pinhole.<br />

ABBERATIONS AND DIFFRACTION<br />

The pupil plays a complex role in relation to image quality <strong>and</strong> hence to acuity. Three major<br />

physical factors are related to pupil size - can a point of light be imaged as a point on the<br />

retina?<br />

Spherical Aberration<br />

In any simple spherical lens the edges of the lens refract more than the centre. Therefore a<br />

distant point can not be imaged as a point.<br />

(Human cornea is actually somewhat flatter at edge; consider effects of hard contact lenses<br />

with large pupils).<br />

Diffraction<br />

Points of light are actually imaged as concentric bright/dark rings. Diffraction effect is<br />

greater with smaller pupils.<br />

Chromatic Aberration<br />

Short wave length light is refracted more than long wave lengths.<br />

Therefore given a white light, different wavelengths will be imaged at different depths.<br />

DEMONSTRATION 4. BLUE-FREE ZONE IN CENTRAL FOVEA<br />

DIRECTIONALITY OF VISION<br />

Visual world is imaged on the retina as in a camera (left-right; up-down inversion).<br />

Yet we locate directions accurately relative to our position in space.<br />

DEMONSTRATION 5. PRESSURE PHOSPHENES<br />

Press gently with a cotton swab on the cornea of your eye. Can you see a pale flash of light?<br />

Where does it seem to come from?<br />

Schlodtmann's Experiment <strong>and</strong> the innate nature of visual direction.<br />

(Keep in mind the orderly relationship between retinal position <strong>and</strong> visual direction: How is<br />

it that we can adapt to optical disruption of direction such as in inverting or lateral-displacing<br />

prisms?)<br />

RETINAL VESSELS AND THE STABILIZED IMAGE<br />

Retinal receptors are supplied by way of the choroidal circulation <strong>and</strong> the pigment<br />

epithelium.<br />

Inner nuclear <strong>and</strong> ganglion cells by way of central retinal artery.<br />

Why don't you see these vessels since they lie in the path of the light?<br />

13


Probably because their shadows are stabilized on the retina.<br />

Therefore if we illuminate eccentrically, we might see them.<br />

DEMONSTRATION 6. PURKINJE VESSEL FIGURE<br />

Illuminate eye through the sclera with a penlight. It is now possible to see an important point<br />

about the position of retinal vessels relative to the fovea.<br />

DEMONSTRATION 7. AFTER IMAGE IN FOVEA WITH VESSELS<br />

Rods have lower thresholds <strong>and</strong> different spectral sensitivity than cones. Purkinje observed<br />

the relative brightness of the rose <strong>and</strong> its leaves in his garden.<br />

DEMONSTRATION 8. RED AND BLUE: BRIGHTNESS IN DIFFERENT LEVELS<br />

OF ILLUMINATION<br />

SPEED OF RESPONSE<br />

Cones work more rapidly: Activate ganglion cells sooner <strong>and</strong> the message reaches the brain<br />

sooner. Even within the photopic range, lower illumination levels lead to slower response of<br />

receptors <strong>and</strong> ganglion cells.<br />

DEMONSTRATION 9. REACTION-TIME UNDER CONDITIONS OF DIM AND<br />

BRIGHT ILLUMINATION<br />

Two illusions induced by differences in latency of response.<br />

DEMONSTRATION 10. FLUTTERING HEARTS<br />

A more subtle effect can be seen binocularly.<br />

DEMONSTRATION 11. PULFRICH EFFECT<br />

Object moves back <strong>and</strong> forth<br />

Seen as a disparity by the two eyes. Therefore interpreted as depth hence<br />

elliptical orbit.<br />

PERSISTENCE OF ROD AFTER IMAGES<br />

DEMONSTRATION 12. FLASH AFTER 5 MINUTES DARK ADAPTATION<br />

CENTRAL COURSE OF OPTIC FIBRES WITHIN RETINA<br />

Ganglion cells fires <strong>and</strong> axon conducts centrally. First along<br />

retinal surface <strong>and</strong> thence, through to the brain.<br />

Transmission via unmyelinated optic nerve fibres.<br />

DEMONSTRATION 13. BLUE ARCS<br />

(1) Why is it shaped as it is?<br />

(2) Why coloured blue?<br />

STABILITY OF THE VISUAL WORLD: EFFERENCE COPY AND THE ILLUSION OF<br />

MOVEMENT<br />

When we sweep our eyes over the scene in front of us it appears to remain stable. Why? Not<br />

necessarily because it is stable.<br />

DEMONSTRATION 14. JIGGLING YOUR EYE WORLD MOVES<br />

Helmholtz Interpretation: Signal from brain nulls apparent movements of the world when we<br />

voluntarily move our eyes.<br />

(Note Not brought into play during vestibular induced movements).<br />

THERE EXIST A PARALLEL SET OF EXPERIMENTS WHICH REINFORCE THE<br />

VIEW<br />

Suppose you have a stable image on your retina <strong>and</strong> will the eyes to move. What happens?<br />

Conversely, if you jiggle your eye, the after-image remains stable.<br />

DEMONSTRATION 15. ACTIVE AND PASSIVE MOVEMENT OF THE EYE WITH<br />

AN AFTER IMAGE ON FOVEA<br />

EYE MOVEMENTS AND VISUAL CONTROL OF MOVEMENT: CHANGING THE<br />

14


IMAGE AND THE MODIFICATION OF VISUALLY INDUCED RESPONSES<br />

The function of vision is to guide movement. Either the whole body, individual limbs, or the<br />

eyes themselves.<br />

Some systematics about movements of the eye.<br />

DEMONSTRATION 16. SMOOTH PURSUIT VS. SACCADIC MOVEMENT<br />

Observe your partner 1) following a smoothly moving target<br />

2) trying to replicate that movement<br />

Only saccade is under voluntary control.<br />

DEMONSTRATION 17. ACCOMMODATION AND VERGENCE MOVEMENT<br />

(pupil size decreases)<br />

Ask subject to view a distant target: observe eyes <strong>and</strong> pupil as you bring it closer to their<br />

face.<br />

VESTIBULO-OCULAR REFLEX<br />

DEMONSTRATION 18. SHAKE HAND WITH HEAD STILL; SHAKE HEAD WITH<br />

HAND STILL<br />

also READ TEXT WHILE SHAKING HEAD OR BODY<br />

LATERALLY DISPLACING PRISM<br />

DEMONSTRATION 19.<br />

Point at target Adapt: Mispoint when prism removed.<br />

Argument that effect is Not Visual (1) Doesn't transfer between arms<br />

(2) Transfer to pointing to a sound source<br />

Vision in conflict with propioception.<br />

DEMONSTRATION 20. VISUAL CAPTURE<br />

15

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!