25.03.2013 Views

Half a Century of Research on the Stroop Effect: An Integrative Review

Half a Century of Research on the Stroop Effect: An Integrative Review

Half a Century of Research on the Stroop Effect: An Integrative Review

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Psychological Bulletin Copyright 1991 by <strong>the</strong> American Psychological Associati<strong>on</strong>, Inc.<br />

199 l, Vol. 109, No. 2, 163-203 0033-2909/91/$3.00<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Half</str<strong>on</strong>g> a <str<strong>on</strong>g>Century</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>Research</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> <strong>Effect</strong>: <strong>An</strong> <strong>Integrative</strong> <strong>Review</strong><br />

Colin M. MacLeod<br />

Divisi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Life Sciences<br />

University <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Tor<strong>on</strong>to, Scarborough Campus<br />

Scarborough, Ontario, Canada<br />

The literature <strong>on</strong> interference in <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> Color-Word Task, covering over 50 years and some 400<br />

studies, is organized and reviewed. In so doing, a set <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>l 8 reliable empirical findings is isolated that<br />

must be captured by any successful <strong>the</strong>ory <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> effect. Existing <strong>the</strong>oretical positi<strong>on</strong>s are<br />

summarized and evaluated in view <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> this critical evidence and <strong>the</strong> 2 major candidate <strong>the</strong>ories--rel-<br />

ative speed <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing and automaticity <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reading--are found to be wanting. It is c<strong>on</strong>cluded that<br />

recent <strong>the</strong>ories placing <strong>the</strong> explanatory weight <strong>on</strong> parallel processing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> irrelevant and <strong>the</strong><br />

relevant dimensi<strong>on</strong>s are likely to be more successful than are earlier <strong>the</strong>ories attempting to locate a<br />

single bottleneck in attenti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

In 1935, J. R. <strong>Stroop</strong> published his landmark article <strong>on</strong> atten-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong> and interference, an article more influential now than it<br />

was <strong>the</strong>n. Why has <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> task c<strong>on</strong>tinued to fascinate us?<br />

Perhaps <strong>the</strong> task is seen as tapping into <strong>the</strong> primitive operati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> cogniti<strong>on</strong>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>fering clues to <strong>the</strong> fundamental process <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> atten-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong>. Perhaps <strong>the</strong> robustness <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> phenomen<strong>on</strong> provides a<br />

special challenge to decipher. Toge<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong>se are powerful at-<br />

tracti<strong>on</strong>s in a field <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> complex phenomena where <strong>the</strong> most sub-<br />

tie variati<strong>on</strong> may exert a dramatic effect.<br />

In writing this article, I have two primary intenti<strong>on</strong>s. The<br />

first is to create a tax<strong>on</strong>omy <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> empirical work <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong><br />

task and its analogs. At least 70% <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> more than 700 <strong>Stroop</strong>-<br />

related articles in <strong>the</strong> literature have been published since <strong>the</strong><br />

reviews by Dyer (1973c) and Jensen and Rohwer (1966). Fur<strong>the</strong>r-<br />

more, Dyer's <strong>the</strong>oretical framework now seems in need <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> re-<br />

c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong>, so that his review no l<strong>on</strong>ger provides <strong>the</strong> optimal<br />

organizati<strong>on</strong>. The sec<strong>on</strong>d aim, <strong>the</strong>n, is to survey existing <strong>the</strong>-<br />

ories, showing <strong>the</strong> critical weaknesses in <strong>the</strong> standard explana-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong>s and dispelling some myths that have arisen. I argue for a<br />

new <strong>the</strong>oretical framework for <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> effect, informed by<br />

c<strong>on</strong>temporary cognitive psychology. This framework is more<br />

comprehensive than many earlier experiment-specific hypo<strong>the</strong>-<br />

ses, and suggests directi<strong>on</strong>s in which future research might<br />

pr<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>itably proceed.<br />

This research was supported by Natural Sciences and Engineering<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Research</str<strong>on</strong>g> Council <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Canada Grant A7459. I am grateful to <strong>the</strong> Ontario<br />

Science Centre for providing testing facilities and to <strong>the</strong>ir staff for<br />

assistance. I owe a c<strong>on</strong>siderable debt to E J. Lootsteen, who did <strong>the</strong><br />

bulk <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> initial work locating articles for me. I thank J<strong>on</strong>athan<br />

Cohen, Kevin Dunbar, and Gord<strong>on</strong> Logan for <strong>the</strong>ir careful readings <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

<strong>the</strong> <strong>the</strong>oretical secti<strong>on</strong>. I am especially grateful to John Flowers and an<br />

an<strong>on</strong>ymous reviewer for <strong>the</strong>ir yeoman work as reviewers.<br />

Corresp<strong>on</strong>dence c<strong>on</strong>cerning this article should be addressed to<br />

Colin M. MacLeod, Divisi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Life Sciences, University <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Tor<strong>on</strong>to,<br />

Scarborough Campus, Scarborough, Ontario Canada M1C IA4. Elec-<br />

tr<strong>on</strong>ic mail may be sent to macleod@lake.scar.utor<strong>on</strong>to.ca.<br />

163<br />

Colors, Words, and Interference:<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Research</str<strong>on</strong>g> From 1886 to 1935<br />

The <str<strong>on</strong>g>Half</str<strong>on</strong>g>-<str<strong>on</strong>g>Century</str<strong>on</strong>g> Before <strong>Stroop</strong><br />

The roots <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>Stroop</strong>'s research are evident 50 years earlier in<br />

<strong>the</strong> work <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> James McKeen Cattell (1886). In a doctoral project<br />

supervised by Wilhelm Wundt, Cattell reported that objects<br />

(and colors) took l<strong>on</strong>ger to name aloud than <strong>the</strong> corresp<strong>on</strong>ding<br />

words took to read aloud. Saying "red" to a patch <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> color was<br />

slower than saying "red" to <strong>the</strong> word red. His explanati<strong>on</strong> is<br />

strikingly modern: "This is because, in <strong>the</strong> case <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words and<br />

letters, <strong>the</strong> associati<strong>on</strong> between <strong>the</strong> idea and name has taken<br />

place so <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>ten that <strong>the</strong> process has become automatic, whereas<br />

in <strong>the</strong> case <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> colors and pictures we must by a voluntary effort<br />

choose <strong>the</strong> name" (1886, p. 65). Cattelrs automatic/voluntary<br />

distincti<strong>on</strong> str<strong>on</strong>gly influenced psychologists <strong>the</strong>n (e.g., James,<br />

1890, p. 559; Quantz, 1897) and c<strong>on</strong>tinues to do so (e.g., Posner<br />

& Snyder, 1975; Schneider & Shiffrin, 1977).<br />

Most early research (e.g., Bills, 1931; Brian & Goodenough,<br />

1929; Gails, 1922) tested <strong>the</strong> intuitive differential-practice ac-<br />

count for CatteU's observati<strong>on</strong> (cf. Quantz, 1897, p. 10). I will<br />

focus <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> few most relevant to <strong>Stroop</strong>'s work. Brown<br />

(1915) predicted that ink-color naming would benefit from ex-<br />

tended practice more than would color word reading because<br />

color naming was initially <strong>the</strong> less practiced skill. Instead, he<br />

found that <strong>the</strong> ratio <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> two times remained quite c<strong>on</strong>stant<br />

with practice. He c<strong>on</strong>cluded that differential practice could not<br />

explain Cattell's findings.1<br />

Lund (1927) disagreed with Brown, and held that <strong>the</strong> differ-<br />

ential practice hypo<strong>the</strong>sis was quite viable. Lund found that<br />

In Experiment 2, Brown printed c<strong>on</strong>gruent words or parts <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words<br />

<strong>on</strong> color backgrounds. C<strong>on</strong>trary to his expectati<strong>on</strong>, such "assistance"<br />

was actually detrimental to color naming. On this basis, Brown argued<br />

that <strong>the</strong> two processes--reading words and naming colors--did not<br />

overlap. This is <strong>the</strong> first published instance where color and word co-<br />

occurred, and where <strong>the</strong> word interfered with naming <strong>the</strong> color.<br />

Strangely, <strong>the</strong> interference appeared in <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>gruent c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>.


164 COLIN M. MACLEOD<br />

children younger than reading age were faster <strong>on</strong> color naming<br />

than <strong>on</strong> word reading, even with careful assurance that <strong>the</strong>y<br />

knew <strong>the</strong> stimulus words. Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, for <strong>on</strong>e 5-year-old<br />

child, 4 weeks <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> alternating practice <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> two tasks suggested<br />

a comm<strong>on</strong> asymptote. However, a larger scale developmental<br />

study c<strong>on</strong>ducted by Lig<strong>on</strong> (1932) again questi<strong>on</strong>ed <strong>the</strong> differen-<br />

tial-practice c<strong>on</strong>cept. Both color-naming and color word-read-<br />

ing times improved across Grades I through 9, yet <strong>the</strong> differ-<br />

ence between <strong>the</strong> skills remained unchanged. Lig<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tended<br />

that a "comm<strong>on</strong> factor" improvedmperhaps time to articulate<br />

<strong>the</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>sembut that <strong>the</strong> tasks were o<strong>the</strong>rwise independent.<br />

In <strong>the</strong> same year as his classic article, <strong>Stroop</strong> (1935a) dem<strong>on</strong>-<br />

strated that Lig<strong>on</strong>'s data did show differential improvement<br />

with age, perfectly in accord with <strong>the</strong> differential-practice idea.<br />

<strong>Stroop</strong> also faulted Lig<strong>on</strong>'s correlati<strong>on</strong>al analysis <strong>on</strong> statistical<br />

grounds. Given Lund's criticism <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Brown, <strong>Stroop</strong> (1935a) advo-<br />

cated <strong>the</strong> differential-practice hypo<strong>the</strong>sis, as indeed did Dyer<br />

(1973c) four decades later.<br />

O<strong>the</strong>r studies <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>fered alternative views <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Cattelrs well-repli-<br />

cated observati<strong>on</strong>. Hollingworth (1912, 1915, 1923) suggested<br />

that word reading required <strong>on</strong>ly articulati<strong>on</strong>, but that color<br />

naming demanded articulati<strong>on</strong> plus associati<strong>on</strong>. Brown (1915)<br />

and Lig<strong>on</strong> (1932) maintained that both tasks involved two pro-<br />

cesses but with a different associati<strong>on</strong> element for each test.<br />

Garrett and Lemm<strong>on</strong> (1924) held that color naming was l<strong>on</strong>ger<br />

because <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> an interference factor, which <strong>the</strong>y failed to specify.<br />

Perhaps <strong>the</strong>y had in mind what Peters<strong>on</strong>, Lanier, and Walker<br />

(1925) suggested: that many resp<strong>on</strong>ses might be c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed to<br />

a single color, but <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e resp<strong>on</strong>se was c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed to a single<br />

word. Telford (1930), a student <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Peters<strong>on</strong>'s, also adopted this<br />

interpretati<strong>on</strong>. <strong>An</strong> earlier variati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> this was put forth by<br />

Woodworth and Wells (1911, p. 52), who argued that <strong>the</strong> few<br />

color names typically used in <strong>the</strong>se experiments "all are equally<br />

ready and get in <strong>on</strong>e ano<strong>the</strong>r's way."<br />

It is difficult to believe that no <strong>on</strong>e thought to combine colors<br />

and words until <strong>Stroop</strong> (1935b). 2 It may help to place <strong>Stroop</strong>'s<br />

insight in c<strong>on</strong>text to realize that he developed his color-word<br />

task less from an interest in stimulus-naming time--<strong>the</strong> moti-<br />

vati<strong>on</strong> for most prior researchmthan from an interest in inter-<br />

ference between c<strong>on</strong>flicting processes.<br />

<strong>Stroop</strong>~ Classic Article<br />

Elsewhere (MacLeod, in press) I have written a biographical<br />

sketch <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> John Ridley <strong>Stroop</strong>; I do not recapitulate that here.<br />

However, before discussing <strong>the</strong> vast literature fostered by<br />

<strong>Stroop</strong>'s dissertati<strong>on</strong>, I will briefly outline his lead article in <strong>the</strong><br />

December 1935 issue <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psychol-<br />

ogy His basic c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong> is correctly reported and widely un-<br />

derstood, but what he actually did has been somewhat trans-<br />

formed in sec<strong>on</strong>dary sources. <strong>Stroop</strong>'s c<strong>on</strong>temporary, Bartlett<br />

(1932), would probably not be surprised by <strong>the</strong> transformati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

I simply wish to refresh our memories with <strong>the</strong> original study,<br />

<strong>Stroop</strong> was c<strong>on</strong>cerned with how best to explain interference.<br />

With Peters<strong>on</strong>, his dissertati<strong>on</strong> supervisor, <strong>Stroop</strong> had been<br />

engaged in studies <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> color naming versus word reading, and hit<br />

up<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> idea <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a compound stimulus where <strong>the</strong> word was<br />

inc<strong>on</strong>gruent with <strong>the</strong> ink color. His two major questi<strong>on</strong>s were<br />

what effect each dimensi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> compound stimulus would<br />

have <strong>on</strong> trying to name <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r dimensi<strong>on</strong>, and what effect<br />

practice would have <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> observed interference.<br />

Experiment I examined <strong>the</strong> effect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> incompatible ink colors<br />

<strong>on</strong> reading words aloud. <strong>Stroop</strong> used five words and <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

matching ink colors: red, blue, green, brown, and purple. For<br />

<strong>the</strong> experimental c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, each ink color appeared twice in<br />

each row and column <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a 10 × 10 stimulus card. Each word<br />

appeared twice <strong>on</strong> every line, equally <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>ten in each <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r<br />

four ink colors. The stimuli appeared in reverse order <strong>on</strong> a<br />

sec<strong>on</strong>d card. The c<strong>on</strong>trol-c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> cards were each identical to<br />

<strong>on</strong>e <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> experimental cards but in black ink <strong>on</strong>ly. After a<br />

10-item practice card, all 70 subjects were tested <strong>on</strong> all four<br />

cards, half in <strong>the</strong> order <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> C<strong>on</strong>trol 1, Experimental 2, Experi-<br />

mental 1, C<strong>on</strong>trol 2, and half in <strong>the</strong> reverse order. Subjects were<br />

to read <strong>the</strong> words aloud as quickly as possible, leaving no errors<br />

uncorrected.<br />

The top row <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Table 1 presents <strong>the</strong> data from Experiment 1.<br />

Although subjects averaged 2.3 s l<strong>on</strong>ger to read <strong>the</strong> 100 words<br />

<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> experimental cards, this 5.6% increase was far from sig-<br />

nificant. I recently replicated this experiment: My data for 50<br />

subjects appear in <strong>the</strong> bottom row <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Table 1, and are remark-<br />

ably c<strong>on</strong>sistent with <strong>Stroop</strong>'s data. There was no interference<br />

from inc<strong>on</strong>gruent colors in reading words, F(I, 49) = 1.86,<br />

MSe = 2.45, p = .17.<br />

In Experiment 2, <strong>the</strong> task was switched to naming <strong>the</strong> colors<br />

aloud. The c<strong>on</strong>trol cards had <strong>the</strong> ink colors in <strong>the</strong> same order as<br />

<strong>the</strong> experimental cards; solid color squares were substituted for<br />

words) The experimental cards and <strong>the</strong> procedure were identi-<br />

cal to Experiment 1, except that twice <strong>the</strong> average resp<strong>on</strong>se time<br />

per item was added to a subject's total for every uncorrected<br />

error, a procedure <strong>Stroop</strong> admitted was"arbitrary." The data <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

<strong>the</strong> 100 subjects in this experiment are presented in <strong>the</strong> top row<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Table 2.<br />

Subjects averaged 47 s l<strong>on</strong>ger to name ink colors <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> inc<strong>on</strong>-<br />

gruent words than solid-color squares. <strong>Stroop</strong> described this<br />

74% increase as a"marked interference effect"; 99% <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> times<br />

<strong>on</strong> experimental cards exceeded those <strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>trol cards. I also<br />

replicated this experiment using 40 subjects. My data, in <strong>the</strong><br />

bottom row <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Table 2, again duplicate those <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>Stroop</strong>; my<br />

slightly lower values are perhaps due to my not using <strong>the</strong> arbi-<br />

trary correcti<strong>on</strong> for errors. There was highly significant interfer-<br />

ence from inc<strong>on</strong>gruent words in naming colors, F(I, 39) =<br />

363.65, MS~ = 99.36, p < .001.<br />

<strong>Stroop</strong>'s little-known third experiment was more compli-<br />

cated. Thirty-two subjects named <strong>the</strong> ink colors <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> incompati-<br />

ble words for 8 days. Over days, color-naming times decreased<br />

16.8 s from 49.6 s to 32.8 s (for a card <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 50 words). Thus,<br />

interference from incompatible words appeared to decrease<br />

2 According to Jensen and Rohwer (1966), Jaensch (1929) was <strong>the</strong><br />

first to combine <strong>the</strong> word and color stimuli in a potentially c<strong>on</strong>flicting<br />

situati<strong>on</strong>. Perhaps, <strong>the</strong>n, Jaensch should receive at least partial credit,<br />

but <strong>the</strong> "Jaensch-<strong>Stroop</strong> effect" is an extremely unwieldy label.<br />

3 <strong>Stroop</strong> changed <strong>the</strong> color-naming c<strong>on</strong>trol-c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> cards from Ex-<br />

periment 2 to Experiment 3. The solid-color squares were replaced by a<br />

n<strong>on</strong>alphanumeric character, which, in <strong>Stroop</strong>'s view, was more like a<br />

letter in appearance and more easily colored to match <strong>the</strong> shades <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong><br />

letters <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> experimental cards. The stimulus <strong>Stroop</strong> chose was <strong>the</strong><br />

swastika.


Table 1<br />

Experiment 1: Mean Times (in Sec<strong>on</strong>ds With Standard<br />

Deviati<strong>on</strong>s [SD] ) for Reading Color Words in <strong>the</strong> Experimental<br />

C<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> (Incompatible Colored Inks) and in <strong>the</strong> C<strong>on</strong>trol<br />

C<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> (Black Ink Only)<br />

Experimental: C<strong>on</strong>trol:<br />

words in color words in black<br />

Experiment Sample size M SD M SD<br />

<strong>Stroop</strong> (1935b) 70 43.30 6.15 41.00 4.84<br />

MacLeod (1986) 50 41.58 6.98 41.16 7.12<br />

with practice. However, <strong>the</strong>re may be a general practice or learn-<br />

ing-to-learn effect here: Ink color-naming baselines were not<br />

collected each day.<br />

Experiment 3 also explored <strong>the</strong> impact <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> practicing color<br />

naming <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> development <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interference in word reading.<br />

Comparis<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a pretest and a posttest where subjects read<br />

words in inc<strong>on</strong>gruent colors showed that <strong>the</strong> intervening 8 days<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> practice introduced interference into word reading (from<br />

19.4 s before to 34.8 s after), but that this newly developed inter-<br />

ference quickly disappeared in a sec<strong>on</strong>d posttest (22.0 s). This<br />

was <strong>the</strong> first report <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> what is now called <strong>the</strong> "reverse <strong>Stroop</strong><br />

effect"<br />

Overall, <strong>Stroop</strong> took his experiments to show that differential<br />

practice did <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>fer a reas<strong>on</strong>able account <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> asymmetrical<br />

interference pattern he obtained, and accepted <strong>the</strong> Peters<strong>on</strong> et<br />

al. (1925) explanati<strong>on</strong> that words evoked a single reading re-<br />

sp<strong>on</strong>se, whereas colors evoked multiple resp<strong>on</strong>ses, <strong>the</strong>reby<br />

making naming colors slower than reading words. Indeed, in<br />

his 1938 article, <strong>Stroop</strong> directly tested this idea and reported<br />

support for it.<br />

The misc<strong>on</strong>cepti<strong>on</strong>s about <strong>Stroop</strong>'s work are subtle but note-<br />

worthy. People <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>ten talk as if <strong>the</strong>re was a single experiment<br />

c<strong>on</strong>trasting four c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s; in fact, he performed two separate<br />

experiments. Frequently, too, his word-reading data (Experi-<br />

ment 1) go unmenti<strong>on</strong>ed. I have even heard <strong>Stroop</strong> credited<br />

with a compatible c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>--color words in c<strong>on</strong>gruent ink<br />

colors--which he never used. He also never timed single stim-<br />

uli; his experiments used <strong>on</strong>ly lists <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> stimuli. On <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r<br />

hand, he did observe reverse <strong>Stroop</strong> interference first. Although<br />

his <strong>the</strong>oretical account did not menti<strong>on</strong> automaticity, it did rely<br />

<strong>on</strong> a primitive type <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> resp<strong>on</strong>se competiti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

In summary, <strong>Stroop</strong>'s article was a careful treatment <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> an<br />

interference phenomen<strong>on</strong>. His scholarly acknowledgment <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

his intellectual precursors, his succinct data analysis, and his<br />

clear writing all c<strong>on</strong>tributed to <strong>the</strong> article's influence. The reli-<br />

ability, size, and apparent simplicity <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> effect also no doubt<br />

captured <strong>the</strong> interest <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> later investigators. I now c<strong>on</strong>sider what<br />

<strong>the</strong>se investigators have found out about <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> effect since<br />

his dissertati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Investigating <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> <strong>Effect</strong>:<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Research</str<strong>on</strong>g> From 1935 to 1989<br />

The set <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> articles <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> effect numbers in excess <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

700. At least 300 <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>se feature <strong>Stroop</strong> interference as a mea-<br />

THE STROOP EFFECT 165<br />

sure <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> attenti<strong>on</strong> in studying individual differences, drug ef-<br />

fects, and <strong>the</strong> like. Some <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>se may assist <strong>the</strong>ory building in<br />

o<strong>the</strong>r domains, but <strong>the</strong>y are not particularly relevant to a <strong>the</strong>o-<br />

retical account <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> effect. I shall omit <strong>the</strong>se "applied"<br />

studies, leaving <strong>the</strong>m for ano<strong>the</strong>r reviewer. Even with such<br />

pruning, however, it would still not be reas<strong>on</strong>able to discuss all<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> remaining articles in equal detail. Thus, I will describe<br />

pivotal studies <strong>on</strong> a topic in detail and <strong>the</strong>n provide reference<br />

pointers to o<strong>the</strong>r pertinent work as appropriate. The review is<br />

selective, but <strong>the</strong> bibliography is comprehensive.<br />

The studies are divided into five secti<strong>on</strong>s. The first secti<strong>on</strong><br />

examines <strong>the</strong> various versi<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> task, now actually a<br />

family <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> tasks. Next are two secti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> stimulus manipula-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong>s: first those c<strong>on</strong>ducted <strong>on</strong> individual trials and <strong>the</strong>n those<br />

involving experiment-wide variables. The fourth secti<strong>on</strong> sum-<br />

marizes manipulati<strong>on</strong>s pertaining to <strong>the</strong> subject's resp<strong>on</strong>se,<br />

and <strong>the</strong> fifth secti<strong>on</strong> surveys subject-related variables. Each sec-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong> is in turn subdivided in terms <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> sorts <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> manipulati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

involved to allow a particular facet <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> work to be located<br />

easily. Appendix A outlines this organizati<strong>on</strong> and serves as an<br />

index.<br />

Variati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> Procedure<br />

The <strong>Stroop</strong> Color-Word Interference Test<br />

The standard <strong>Stroop</strong> Color-Word Test, sometimes called <strong>the</strong><br />

Serial Color-Word Test, is <strong>the</strong> procedure <strong>Stroop</strong> (1935b) used in<br />

his Experiment 2. Typically, subjects are tested <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong> naming<br />

colors <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> incompatible words and <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>trol patches. Interfer-<br />

ence is expressed as <strong>the</strong> difference between <strong>the</strong> times <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>se<br />

two types <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> cards. Total time per card divided by number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

stimuli <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> card occasi<strong>on</strong>ally is used to estimate time per<br />

stimulus. Jensen and Rohwer (1966) detailed many <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> subtle<br />

shadings <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> this standard procedure, focusing particularly <strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> Thurst<strong>on</strong>e (1944) versi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

It is surprising how few studies investigating interference<br />

have been c<strong>on</strong>cerned at all with <strong>Stroop</strong>'s Experiment 1: reading<br />

words in black versus inc<strong>on</strong>gruent colors. I agree with Smekal<br />

and Dvoracek (1977) that more attenti<strong>on</strong> should be given to this<br />

sec<strong>on</strong>d measure if <strong>on</strong>ly to reduce misinterpretati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> stan-<br />

dard interference measure. This may be especially crucial in<br />

clinical settings and individual-differences studies, where more<br />

c<strong>on</strong>vergence would be helpful.<br />

Table 2<br />

Experiment 2: Mean Times (in Sec<strong>on</strong>ds with Standard<br />

Deviati<strong>on</strong>s [SD]) for Naming Ink Colors in <strong>the</strong> Experimental<br />

C<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> (Incompatible Color Words) and in <strong>the</strong> C<strong>on</strong>trol<br />

C<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> (Solid Color Squares)<br />

C<strong>on</strong>trol:<br />

Experimental: squares in<br />

words in color color<br />

Experiment Sample size M SD M SD<br />

<strong>Stroop</strong> (1935b) 100 110.3 18.8 63.3 10.8<br />

MacLeod (1986) 40 102.27 18.06 59.76 8.09


166 COLIN M. MACLEOD<br />

How should <strong>the</strong> standard color-word test be scored? Jensen<br />

and Rohwer (1966, pp. 45-48) discussed many approaches, but<br />

<strong>the</strong> most prevalent is to calculate a difference score (interfer-<br />

ence card time - pure color card time) following <strong>Stroop</strong><br />

(1935b). 4 Various more complicated scoring systems have been<br />

proposed (e.g., Smith, 1959; Smith & Borg, 1964; Smith & Klein,<br />

1953), but <strong>the</strong>ir use is nowhere near as widespread as <strong>the</strong><br />

straightforward difference score. The same applies to attempts<br />

to deal with errors <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> task (e.g., Gardner, Holzman, Klein,<br />

Lint<strong>on</strong>, & Spence, 1959; Rand, Wapner, Werner, & McFarland,<br />

1963).<br />

Although Sj6berg (1969,1974) was pessimistic about <strong>the</strong> reli-<br />

ability and validity <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> Color-Word Test based <strong>on</strong> his<br />

regressi<strong>on</strong> analyses <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> various clinical populati<strong>on</strong>s, o<strong>the</strong>rs have<br />

been more optimistic. Smith and Nyman (1974) and Sehubo<br />

and Hentschel (1977, 1978) argued that reliability was actually<br />

quite good. Santos and M<strong>on</strong>tgomery (1962) directly examined<br />

reliability, finding it to be good and uninfluenced by events<br />

interpolated between test and retest. Uechi (1972) also reported<br />

high reliability. In <strong>the</strong> most comprehensive study, Jensen (1965)<br />

c<strong>on</strong>cluded that, with multiple administrati<strong>on</strong>s, <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> test<br />

was probably more reliable than any o<strong>the</strong>r psychometric test.<br />

Of course, many studies have centered <strong>on</strong> methodological<br />

issues in <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> task. Zajano, Hoyceanyls, and Ouellette<br />

(1981 ) pointed out that ink color and shape change repeatedly<br />

<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> standard interference card, but <strong>on</strong>ly ink color changes <strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> standard c<strong>on</strong>trol card. Changing shapes <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>trol card<br />

to correct this c<strong>on</strong>found did not alter <strong>the</strong> basic effect, however.<br />

Sichel and Chandler (1969) argued that variati<strong>on</strong> over stimuli<br />

was too great in <strong>the</strong> standard test; <strong>the</strong>ir procedure using pairs <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

stimuli and tighter c<strong>on</strong>trols <strong>on</strong> hue, brightness, and so forth also<br />

left <strong>the</strong> basic pattern intact.<br />

Modificati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong>ly modestly affect its magnitude, not its qual-<br />

itative form. McCown and Arnoult (1981) printed <strong>the</strong> stimuli<br />

vertically versus horiz<strong>on</strong>tally using ei<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> entire color word<br />

or just its first three letters, and reported equivalent interfer-<br />

ence in all cases. Regan (1978) found interference even using<br />

<strong>on</strong>ly <strong>the</strong> first letter <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a c<strong>on</strong>flicting color word (e.g., <strong>the</strong> letter B<br />

in red ink). Nor do instructi<strong>on</strong>al manipulati<strong>on</strong>s have much im-<br />

pact. For example, Peretti (1971 ) found that varying <strong>the</strong> degree<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> competitiveness in instructi<strong>on</strong>s to subjects affected <strong>the</strong>ir per-<br />

formance, but substantial interference appeared in both in-<br />

structi<strong>on</strong>al c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Group versi<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> color-word test originated with Kipnis<br />

and Glickman (1958, 1959a, 1959b, 1962). They had subject<br />

groups view cards c<strong>on</strong>taining 150 stimuli and <strong>the</strong>n write down<br />

<strong>the</strong> first letter <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> each ink color, scoring <strong>the</strong> number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> items<br />

correctly identified in a 3-min interval. Similar schemes were<br />

developed by Uhlmann (1962) and by Podell (1963). Unfortu-<br />

nately, as Kipnis and Glickman (1962) noted, <strong>the</strong>se tests actu-<br />

ally measured clerical speed more than anything else.<br />

Golden (1975) had subjects in a group silently read as many<br />

items as <strong>the</strong>y could <strong>on</strong> both experimental and c<strong>on</strong>trol cards in<br />

45 s. (Although he claimed that performance was equivalent to<br />

<strong>the</strong> same task d<strong>on</strong>e aloud, I do not recommend switching from<br />

overt to covert naming.) Work by Harbes<strong>on</strong> and her colleagues<br />

(Harbes<strong>on</strong>, Kennedy, & Bittner, 1981; Harbes<strong>on</strong>, Krause, Ken-<br />

nedy, & Bittner, 1982) including a group <strong>Stroop</strong> test found indi-<br />

vidual card times to be reas<strong>on</strong>ably reliable, but not <strong>the</strong> derived<br />

difference scores. Thus far, <strong>the</strong>n, use <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> group tests has been<br />

infrequent and <strong>the</strong> results not very compelling.<br />

On <strong>the</strong> whole, <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> Color-Word Test as a psychometric<br />

tool appears to have reas<strong>on</strong>able reliability and validity (al-<br />

though <strong>the</strong> appropriateness <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> any given criteri<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> validity<br />

could be questi<strong>on</strong>ed). Coupled with its ease <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> administrati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

this has made <strong>the</strong> test a widely used diagnostic instrument in its<br />

standard form. Not surprisingly, <strong>the</strong>n, many variati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

test have been developed and tested.<br />

The Individual Stimulus Versi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> Color-Word Task<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Research</str<strong>on</strong>g>ers interested in studying interference wanted a<br />

more analytic methodology whereby individual stimuli could<br />

be presented and timed. Although Dyer (1973c) credited<br />

Dalrymple-Alford and Budayr (1966) with first using this pro-<br />

cedure, Tecce and Dimartino (1965) used it a year earlier to<br />

study shock effects and resp<strong>on</strong>se competiti<strong>on</strong>. However,<br />

Dalrymple-Alford and Budayr's (1966) c<strong>on</strong>cern was with <strong>the</strong><br />

procedural modificati<strong>on</strong> itself, and <strong>the</strong>y found it acceptable.<br />

They did note, though, that <strong>the</strong> standard color-word test intro-<br />

duced list-structure effects in additi<strong>on</strong> to item-specific interfer-<br />

ence.<br />

By <strong>the</strong> time <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Dyer's (1973c) review, quite a few studies had<br />

used this procedure; it has certainly become predominant in<br />

laboratories since <strong>the</strong>n (e.g., Dunbar & MacLeod, 1984; Neill,<br />

1977). Indeed, it was not enough to isolate a single <strong>Stroop</strong> stimu-<br />

lus; investigators <strong>the</strong>n began to modify <strong>the</strong> format <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> that stimu-<br />

lus. For example, Kamlet and Egeth (1969) embossed color<br />

words in white letters <strong>on</strong> colored plastic tape and had subjects<br />

name <strong>the</strong> tape color. They observed interference virtually iden-<br />

tical to that in <strong>the</strong> integrated, word-in-color standard proce-<br />

dure.<br />

To examine laterality and stimulus-integrality effects, Dyer<br />

(1973a) separated word from color, placing <strong>on</strong>e <strong>on</strong> each side <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

<strong>the</strong> fixati<strong>on</strong> point. In like manner, Kahneman and Chajczyk<br />

(1983) placed a word above or below a color patch in <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

studies <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> automaticity. Both studies obtained robust interfer-<br />

ence. Dyer and Severance (1973) also separated word and color,<br />

presenting an incompatible color word 0, 25, 50 or 100 ms be-<br />

fore a color patch. Although <strong>the</strong>y observed interference in every<br />

case, it was about half that <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> normal integrated stimulus.<br />

Their procedure has now become c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al for <strong>the</strong> investi-<br />

gati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> stimulus <strong>on</strong>set asynchr<strong>on</strong>y (SOA) (see later discus-<br />

si<strong>on</strong>). Indeed, all <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>se innovati<strong>on</strong>s are now firmly en-<br />

sc<strong>on</strong>ced, and <strong>the</strong> single-trial (or single-resp<strong>on</strong>se) procedure<br />

dominates <strong>the</strong> field.<br />

Sorting and Matching Versi<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> Color-Word Task<br />

Ra<strong>the</strong>r than naming or reading stimuli aloud, subjects can be<br />

asked to sort stimuli into categories. The originators <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> this<br />

procedure, Tecce and Happ (1964), showed that sorting color-<br />

<strong>on</strong>ly cards was much faster than sorting inc<strong>on</strong>gruent color-<br />

4 Saunders (1980) suggested <strong>the</strong> use <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> logarithm <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> this differ-<br />

ence score <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> curious grounds that willingness to take part in<br />

psychological experiments, especially <strong>Stroop</strong> studies, seems to corre-<br />

late with this measure.


word cards into categories identified by color patches. Dyer<br />

(1973c) w<strong>on</strong>dered what would have happened had <strong>the</strong> piles<br />

been labeled with color words instead. The answer came when<br />

Chmiel (1984) labeled <strong>the</strong> bins both ways (in separate c<strong>on</strong>di-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong>s), and observed both normal <strong>Stroop</strong> interference and re-<br />

verse <strong>Stroop</strong> interference. Martin (1981) also obtained a reverse<br />

<strong>Stroop</strong> effect using <strong>the</strong> card-sorting procedure.<br />

Treisman and Fearnley (1969) had subjects sort cards c<strong>on</strong>-<br />

taining two words, two colors, or a word and a color into <strong>on</strong>e <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

two piles identified by whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> two stimuli <strong>on</strong> a given card<br />

were <strong>the</strong> same or different. When both stimuli were words or<br />

both were colors, sorting was rapid. When <strong>the</strong> stimuli were a<br />

word and a color, sorting was slower, especially when <strong>on</strong>e <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong><br />

two was an inc<strong>on</strong>gruent stimulus.<br />

The sorting task c<strong>on</strong>tinues to appear periodically in <strong>the</strong> litera-<br />

ture (Flowers & Blair, 1976; Flowers & Dutch, 1976; Naish,<br />

1980; Taylor & Clive, 1983; Virzi & Egeth, 1985), sometimes<br />

using materials o<strong>the</strong>rs than colors and words (e.g., Mort<strong>on</strong>,<br />

1969). Because some <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>se findings suggest that <strong>the</strong> sorting<br />

task may not be perfectly analogous to <strong>the</strong> standard <strong>Stroop</strong><br />

task, it will be necessary to compare results from <strong>the</strong> different<br />

procedures directly (as already begun by Taylor & Clive, 1983).<br />

Although a comm<strong>on</strong> cognitive task, most notably in <strong>the</strong> work<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Posner (1978), matching has seen limited use in <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong><br />

situati<strong>on</strong>. Flowers (1975) first used <strong>the</strong> technique to examine<br />

time to match sequentially presented color words to color<br />

patches. If printed in an inc<strong>on</strong>gruent ink color, time to match<br />

<strong>the</strong> word with <strong>the</strong> color patch was slowed (relative to <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>-<br />

gruent or c<strong>on</strong>trol cases). Compt<strong>on</strong> and Flowers (1977) repli-<br />

cated and extended this observati<strong>on</strong>. O<strong>the</strong>rs (e.g., Alivisatos &<br />

Wilding, 1982) have used matching to study interference in<br />

<strong>Stroop</strong> analogs, but <strong>on</strong>ly rarely. There appears to be room for<br />

more research using matching, particularly in attempting to<br />

determine <strong>the</strong> role <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> resp<strong>on</strong>se modality in interference (see<br />

Resp<strong>on</strong>se Modality: Oral Versus Manual).<br />

The Picture-Word Interference Task<br />

Cattell (1886) emphasized <strong>the</strong> naming <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> colors and pictures<br />

being slower than <strong>the</strong> reading aloud <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> corresp<strong>on</strong>ding words<br />

(see also Fraisse, 1969). So it is surprising that <strong>the</strong> picture-word<br />

interference task did not appear until after Dyer's (1973c) re-<br />

view. Hentschel (1973) embedded words inside line drawings<br />

and required subjects to name <strong>the</strong> pictures. Although uncertain<br />

whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> two tests measured precisely <strong>the</strong> same phenome-<br />

n<strong>on</strong>, he validated <strong>the</strong> resulting interference against <strong>the</strong> stan-<br />

dard <strong>Stroop</strong> test.<br />

Hentschel used <strong>the</strong> multiple-stimulus form; more prevalent<br />

today is <strong>the</strong> individual-stimulus versi<strong>on</strong>, originating with Las-<br />

sen (1975) in an unpublished dissertati<strong>on</strong>. He observed <strong>the</strong> larg-<br />

est interference when <strong>the</strong> word preceded <strong>the</strong> picture by 100 ms.<br />

(Note that his parallel study <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> word reading with inc<strong>on</strong>gruent<br />

pictures also dem<strong>on</strong>strated some interference, although this<br />

was unaffected by SOA.)<br />

The task quickly caught <strong>on</strong>. Rosinski, Golink<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>f, and Kukish<br />

(1975) dem<strong>on</strong>strated that inc<strong>on</strong>gruent words printed inside<br />

pictures interfered substantially with picture naming, but that<br />

inc<strong>on</strong>gruent pictures had <strong>on</strong>ly small effects <strong>on</strong> word reading.<br />

As in <strong>the</strong> color-word task, this asymmetry has come to be <strong>the</strong><br />

THE STROOP EFFECT 167<br />

expected pattern (e.g., Glaser & DiJngelh<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>f, 1984; Smith & Ma-<br />

gee, 1980). Golink<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>fand Rosinski (1976) showed that picture-<br />

word interference was unaffected by reading-comprehensi<strong>on</strong><br />

ability. Rosinski (1977) reported that same-category words in-<br />

terfered more with picture naming than did unrelated words or<br />

n<strong>on</strong>sense trigram c<strong>on</strong>trol items. These studies set <strong>the</strong> stage for<br />

subsequent work <strong>on</strong> picture-word interference.<br />

The major advantage <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> picture-word task is its greater<br />

flexibility in allowing many manipulati<strong>on</strong>s not possible with<br />

<strong>the</strong> restricted set <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> colors in <strong>the</strong> color-word task. In compari-<br />

s<strong>on</strong> to pictures-<strong>on</strong>ly, rows <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Xs, or o<strong>the</strong>r c<strong>on</strong>trols (e.g., *#@?%),<br />

inc<strong>on</strong>gruent words c<strong>on</strong>sistently interfere with picture naming.<br />

Also c<strong>on</strong>gruent words speed resp<strong>on</strong>ding (Posnansky & Rayner,<br />

1977; Rayner & Posnansky, 1978; Underwood, 1976). N<strong>on</strong>-<br />

words cause less interference than words (e.g., Lupker, 1982),<br />

and unpr<strong>on</strong>ounceable n<strong>on</strong>words cause less interference than<br />

pr<strong>on</strong>ounceable n<strong>on</strong>words (e.g., Guttentag & Haith, 1978).<br />

More intriguing, perhaps, are <strong>the</strong> findings unique to <strong>the</strong> pic-<br />

ture-word task. C<strong>on</strong>sider first <strong>the</strong> semantic domain. Given <strong>the</strong><br />

numerous dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> semantic facilitati<strong>on</strong> as a result <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

associati<strong>on</strong> (e.g., Meyer & Schvaneveldt, 1976), it is puzzling<br />

that an associated word printed <strong>on</strong> a picture (e.g., <strong>the</strong> word<br />

cheese <strong>on</strong> a picture <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a mouse) does not produce more interfer-<br />

ence in picture naming than does an unassociated word<br />

(Lupker, 1979). Yet a member <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> same semantic category<br />

(e.g., <strong>the</strong> word ankle <strong>on</strong> a picture <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a hand) does produce more<br />

interference than an unrelated word (e.g., Ehri, 1976; Golink<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>f<br />

& Rosinski, 1976), regardless <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> exposure durati<strong>on</strong> (Rayner &<br />

Springer, 1986). Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, although category typicality has<br />

no influence <strong>on</strong> interference (Lupker, 1979), category centrality<br />

apparently does (Job & Rumiati, 1984). Obviously, this is a com-<br />

plicated issue.<br />

There is now a large data base <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> studies manipulating se-<br />

mantic relati<strong>on</strong>s between word and picture (e.g., Babbitt, 1982;<br />

Brys<strong>on</strong>, 1983; Dunbar, 1986; Irwin & Lupker, 1983; Lupker &<br />

Katz, 198 l, 1982; Lupker & Sanders, 1982; Magee, 1982; Rayner<br />

& Springer, 1986; Reiner & Morris<strong>on</strong>, 1983; Smith & Kirsner,<br />

1982; Smith & Magee, 1980; Toma & Tsao, 1985). Many <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>se<br />

are discussed later in more detail. Although a quick glance<br />

might suggest that increasing <strong>the</strong> semantic similarity between<br />

<strong>the</strong> word and <strong>the</strong> picture increases <strong>the</strong> interference in picture<br />

naming, this comforting summary is flawed. For example, it<br />

does not capture <strong>the</strong> difference just noted between categori-<br />

cally and associatively related words. A complete account<br />

awaits fur<strong>the</strong>r research.<br />

A smaller subset <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> picture-word studies has been c<strong>on</strong>cerned<br />

with n<strong>on</strong>semantic relati<strong>on</strong>s. Ehri (1977) dem<strong>on</strong>strated that pic-<br />

ture-naming latencies showed less interference as <strong>the</strong> words<br />

changed from nouns to adjectives to functors, and that functors<br />

did not differ from c<strong>on</strong>trol n<strong>on</strong>sense syllables. Thus, syntactic<br />

functi<strong>on</strong> (or meaningfulness, or both) plays a role roughly in<br />

line with intuiti<strong>on</strong>. However, most variati<strong>on</strong>s have centered <strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> ph<strong>on</strong>emic, graphemic, or orthographic characteristics <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

words. The basic message is that <strong>the</strong>se variati<strong>on</strong>s influence in-<br />

terference, but not as much as semantic manipulati<strong>on</strong>s (e.g.,<br />

Briggs & Underwood, 1982; Posnansky & Rayner, 1977). As<br />

<strong>on</strong>e example <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a n<strong>on</strong>semantic effect, Rayner and Posnansky<br />

(1978) found that n<strong>on</strong>words that resembled words graphemi-<br />

cally (e.g., leuf for leaf) assisted naming <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> corresp<strong>on</strong>ding


168 COLIN M. MACLEOD<br />

picture when presented very briefly (35 ms). With increased<br />

exposure durati<strong>on</strong>, this facilitati<strong>on</strong> gradually became interfer-<br />

ence, although interference was never as great for <strong>the</strong>se look-<br />

alikes as for dissimilar n<strong>on</strong>words. To complicate matters, <strong>the</strong>y<br />

observed that ph<strong>on</strong>emically related n<strong>on</strong>words (e.g., lefe for leaf)<br />

led to increased facilitati<strong>on</strong> with exposure durati<strong>on</strong>, in opposi-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong> to <strong>the</strong> graphemic effect.<br />

Posnansky and Rayner (1978) established that first-letter co-<br />

incidence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> word and <strong>the</strong> picture exerted its effect through<br />

<strong>the</strong> graphemic ra<strong>the</strong>r than <strong>the</strong> ph<strong>on</strong>emic route. Like Rayner<br />

and Posnansky (1978), Lupker (1982) and Underwood and<br />

Briggs (1984) showed that <strong>the</strong> graphemic similarity effect was<br />

due to letters appearing in <strong>the</strong> correct locati<strong>on</strong>, and that <strong>the</strong>y<br />

did not have to overlap in <strong>the</strong> first positi<strong>on</strong> (e.g., <strong>the</strong> word bomb<br />

<strong>on</strong> a picture <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a comb produced <strong>the</strong> effect). (Note that this<br />

observati<strong>on</strong> is c<strong>on</strong>sistent with <strong>the</strong> predicti<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> McClelland<br />

and Rumelhart's, 1981, activati<strong>on</strong> model.)<br />

What do <strong>the</strong>se studies tell us? Lupker (1982; Lupker &<br />

Sanders, 1982) suggests that graphemic effects are a c<strong>on</strong>se-<br />

quence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> easier word retrieval, and are unrelated to <strong>the</strong> re-<br />

sp<strong>on</strong>se-competiti<strong>on</strong> factors underlying <strong>the</strong> basic interference<br />

effect. Rayner and Springer (1986) showed that graphemic simi-<br />

larity between <strong>the</strong> picture and <strong>the</strong> word actually reduced <strong>the</strong><br />

size <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> semantic-category effect. They maintained that <strong>the</strong><br />

bulk <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> interference in <strong>the</strong> picture-word task stems from<br />

semantic evaluati<strong>on</strong>, not resp<strong>on</strong>se competiti<strong>on</strong>. Investigators<br />

seem to agree that <strong>the</strong>re is more than <strong>on</strong>e factor involved in<br />

picture-word interference, but what <strong>the</strong> factors are and how<br />

fundamental each is remain to be worked out.<br />

A critical questi<strong>on</strong> is how directly <strong>the</strong> picture-word and<br />

color-word findings relate to each o<strong>the</strong>r. Given <strong>the</strong> quite similar<br />

patterns outlined, I will discuss <strong>the</strong>m toge<strong>the</strong>r in <strong>the</strong> rest <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> this<br />

article. However, <strong>the</strong> essential work <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> ascertaining empirically<br />

just how similar <strong>the</strong> two tasks are remains to be performed.<br />

This is not a straightforward problem given <strong>the</strong> different sorts <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

questi<strong>on</strong>s being addressed in <strong>the</strong> two domains. One way to<br />

make some inroads would be through an individual-differences<br />

study examining <strong>the</strong> correlati<strong>on</strong> between interference in <strong>the</strong><br />

two tasks. The greater <strong>the</strong> comparability <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> results in <strong>the</strong> two<br />

tasks over <strong>the</strong> same individuals, <strong>the</strong> more c<strong>on</strong>fident we will be<br />

that <strong>the</strong>y measure <strong>the</strong> same cognitive processes.<br />

Auditory <strong>An</strong>alogs <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> Task<br />

If people could selectively filter by modality (as Broadbent,<br />

1958, suggested), <strong>the</strong>n <strong>the</strong>re ought not be much cross-modality<br />

interference. A number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> studies called this predicti<strong>on</strong> into<br />

questi<strong>on</strong> (e.g., Greenwald, 1970), so creati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> an auditory<br />

<strong>Stroop</strong> task was <strong>on</strong>ly a matter <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> time. Credit goes to Hamers<br />

(1973) in her dissertati<strong>on</strong> (see Hamers & Lambert, 1972). Her<br />

compatible c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> required subjects to say "low" to <strong>the</strong> word<br />

low presented at a low pitch (110 Hz) and "high" to <strong>the</strong> word<br />

high presented at a high pitch (175 Hz). In <strong>the</strong> incompatible<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, <strong>the</strong> word at 110 Hz was high and <strong>the</strong> word at 175 Hz<br />

was low. Examining <strong>on</strong>ly her m<strong>on</strong>olingual c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, she re-<br />

ported str<strong>on</strong>g interference: The incompatible case (1,169 ms)<br />

took 138 ms l<strong>on</strong>ger than <strong>the</strong> compatible case (1,031 ms) <strong>on</strong><br />

average.<br />

Unfortunately, as Dyer (1973c) pointed out, we cannot be sure<br />

how much <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> this difference truly is interference caused by in-<br />

c<strong>on</strong>gruent words and how much is facilitati<strong>on</strong> resulting from<br />

c<strong>on</strong>gruent words because <strong>the</strong>re was no neutral-word c<strong>on</strong>trol<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. This omissi<strong>on</strong> is all too frequent in studies <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interfer-<br />

ence and facilitati<strong>on</strong>, complicating interpretati<strong>on</strong>. Without<br />

such a c<strong>on</strong>trol, all that can be examined is <strong>the</strong> difference be-<br />

tween <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>gruent c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> and <strong>the</strong> inc<strong>on</strong>gruent c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong><br />

--<strong>the</strong> sum <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> facilitati<strong>on</strong> and interference, each in unknown<br />

amounts. A sec<strong>on</strong>d problem is closely related: What is <strong>the</strong> ap-<br />

propriate neutral c<strong>on</strong>trol?. I return to this thorny issue later (cf.<br />

J<strong>on</strong>ides & Mack, 1984); for now, let us c<strong>on</strong>tinue examining audi-<br />

tory <strong>Stroop</strong> analogs.<br />

Dyer (1973c) described an unpublished experiment by Dyer<br />

and Mosko wherein subjects had to report <strong>the</strong> name <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong><br />

speaker. Some words spoken were neutral, some were compati-<br />

ble (<strong>the</strong> speaker's name), and some were incompatible (<strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r<br />

speaker's name). They found no interference or facilitati<strong>on</strong>, ap-<br />

parently in c<strong>on</strong>flict with Hamers (1973). However, <strong>the</strong> Hamers<br />

result has been replicated (Cohen & Martin, 1975; Shor, 1975).<br />

In fact, Shor obtained interference in both directi<strong>on</strong>s, although<br />

<strong>the</strong> word interfered more with identifying <strong>the</strong> pitch than vice<br />

versa. Bidirecti<strong>on</strong>al interference is rarely reported (partly be-<br />

cause subjects are rarely required to name both dimensi<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

<strong>the</strong> stimulus), yet it has serious ramificati<strong>on</strong>s for interpreting<br />

<strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> effect.<br />

In an extensi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Hamers's task, McClain (1983c) c<strong>on</strong>trasted<br />

time to identify <strong>the</strong> pitch with time to identify <strong>the</strong> word using<br />

three resp<strong>on</strong>se modes: verbal, butt<strong>on</strong>press, or pitched hum.<br />

Interference occurred <strong>on</strong>ly when <strong>the</strong> to-be-reported dimensi<strong>on</strong><br />

and <strong>the</strong> reporting mode differed (pitch-verbal, pitch-butt<strong>on</strong>,<br />

word-hum), and not when <strong>the</strong>y were compatible (word-verbal,<br />

word-butt<strong>on</strong>, pitch-hum). She implicated resp<strong>on</strong>se modality as<br />

crucial in interference, an issue discussed under Resp<strong>on</strong>se Mo-<br />

dality: Oral Versus Manual.<br />

O<strong>the</strong>r auditory procedures have been reported as well.<br />

Pieters (1981) used <strong>the</strong> auditory words left or right to <strong>the</strong> left or<br />

right <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> subject, and obtained interference in reporting <strong>the</strong><br />

spatial locati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a c<strong>on</strong>flicting word. Green and Barber (1981,<br />

1983) obtained an auditory <strong>Stroop</strong> effect when subjects had to<br />

judge a speaker's gender in c<strong>on</strong>flict versus no-c<strong>on</strong>flict situa-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong>s. Hatta and Kirsner (1983) reported a similar result, which<br />

also held with a butt<strong>on</strong>press ra<strong>the</strong>r than a vocal resp<strong>on</strong>se. Ap-<br />

parently, <strong>the</strong> Dyer and Mosko finding is <strong>the</strong> outlier.<br />

Most recently, Zakay and Glicksohn (1985) used musical<br />

notes or <strong>the</strong> names <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> those notes printed at c<strong>on</strong>gruent or inc<strong>on</strong>-<br />

gruent locati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> staff. Subjects resp<strong>on</strong>ded ei<strong>the</strong>r orally or<br />

<strong>on</strong> a piano keyboard. Zakay and Glicksohn implicated both<br />

stimulus-resp<strong>on</strong>se compatibility and <strong>the</strong> relati<strong>on</strong> between <strong>the</strong><br />

dimensi<strong>on</strong>s as important, suggesting in fact that <strong>the</strong>se effects<br />

were additive.<br />

All <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>se authors explicitly or implicitly argue for a close<br />

analogy between <strong>the</strong>ir task and <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> task. As with pic-<br />

ture-word interference, though, this c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong> has not been<br />

firmly established. Indeed, it may be somewhat less clear-cut<br />

here. Still <strong>the</strong> effects are generally qualitatively similar, suggest-<br />

ing generalizability <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> interference phenomen<strong>on</strong>.<br />

O<strong>the</strong>r <strong>An</strong>alogs <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> Task<br />

These variants <strong>on</strong>ly hint at <strong>the</strong> ingenuity <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> investigators in<br />

creating interference tasks. Many <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>rs involve two (or


more) verbal stimuli <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> same trial (or <strong>on</strong> successive trials),<br />

where <strong>on</strong>e is <strong>the</strong> target and <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r is <strong>the</strong> distractor. These<br />

tend to be thought <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> in <strong>the</strong> literature more as priming studies,<br />

outside <strong>the</strong> domain <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> this review. As <strong>on</strong>e illustrati<strong>on</strong>, though, a<br />

widely used procedure is <strong>the</strong> "flanker" task (e.g., Eriksen &<br />

Eriksen, 1974; Eriksen & H<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>fman, 1972; Flowers & Wilcox,<br />

1982) in which irrelevant letters or words are presented adjacent<br />

to <strong>the</strong> target.<br />

Eriksen and Eriksen (1974) showed that, for two resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

sets <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> letters (H and K vs. S and C), irrelevant flankers from <strong>the</strong><br />

wr<strong>on</strong>g set interfered with making <strong>the</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>se. This seems to<br />

be an attenti<strong>on</strong>al-selecti<strong>on</strong> problem, because presenting <strong>the</strong><br />

flankers in a different color reduced <strong>the</strong> interference (Harms &<br />

Bundesen, 1983). Hatta, Hatae, and Kirsner (1984) c<strong>on</strong>trasted<br />

English m<strong>on</strong>olinguals to English-Japanese bilinguals in identi-<br />

fying English letters, and found that flanker characters from<br />

ei<strong>the</strong>r language interfered for <strong>the</strong> bilinguals, but not for <strong>the</strong><br />

m<strong>on</strong>olinguals.<br />

O<strong>the</strong>r investigators adapted this procedure for studying word<br />

ra<strong>the</strong>r than letter processing. Dallas and Merikle (1976) showed<br />

that presence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>a distractor word influenced time to name <strong>the</strong><br />

target word, with slower naming when <strong>the</strong> distractor was unas-<br />

sociated. Shaffer and LaBerge (1979) replicated <strong>the</strong> Eriksen and<br />

Eriksen (1974) pattern using categorized words instead <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> let-<br />

ters. Guttentag, Haith, Goodman, and Hauch (1984) used <strong>the</strong><br />

technique with bilinguals to show that flanker words interfered<br />

even when <strong>the</strong>y were not in <strong>the</strong> same language as <strong>the</strong> target,<br />

c<strong>on</strong>trasting somewhat with <strong>the</strong> Hatta et al. 0984) result for<br />

letters, perhaps because <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> semantic element. The bilingual<br />

<strong>Stroop</strong> situati<strong>on</strong> is treated in more depth under Language Dif-<br />

ferences: The Case <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> Bilingual.<br />

The growing literature <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> flanker task has been extended<br />

to <strong>the</strong> color-word and picture-word situati<strong>on</strong>s by Kahneman<br />

and Chajczyk (1983) and Glaser and Glaser (1989). Several stud-<br />

ies also have investigated aspects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> meaning using a priming<br />

procedure in a <strong>Stroop</strong>-like task (e.g., C<strong>on</strong>rad, 1974; Henik,<br />

Friedrich, & Kellogg, 1983; Oden & Spira, 1983; Regan, 1977,<br />

1981; Warren, 1972, 1974), but <strong>the</strong>se are all saved for <strong>the</strong> more<br />

extensive discussi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> semantic variati<strong>on</strong> later.<br />

Most relevant interference experiments have used words as<br />

<strong>on</strong>e dimensi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> two-dimensi<strong>on</strong>al stimulus, such as when<br />

Warren and Lasher (1974) had subjects identify <strong>the</strong> type f<strong>on</strong>ts <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

words that were in ei<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>gruent or <strong>the</strong> inc<strong>on</strong>gruent f<strong>on</strong>t<br />

(e.g., <strong>the</strong> word bold typed in modern, n<strong>on</strong>bold f<strong>on</strong>t), obtaining<br />

<strong>the</strong> expected interference pattern. However, shapes have been<br />

used occasi<strong>on</strong>ally in an effort to eliminate words altoge<strong>the</strong>r (e.g.,<br />

Irwin, 1978; Redding & Gerjets, 1977; Shor, 197 l). Thus, Hents-<br />

chel (1973) developed a figure-word interference test in <strong>the</strong><br />

same article in which he introduced <strong>the</strong> picture-word task.<br />

When geometrical shapes surrounded inc<strong>on</strong>gruent words (e.g.,<br />

a square around <strong>the</strong> word circle), interference occurred for nam-<br />

ing <strong>the</strong> shape.<br />

Compt<strong>on</strong> and Flowers (1977) had subjects match words to<br />

geometric shapes presented shortly after <strong>the</strong> words. Matching<br />

speed was slowed by inc<strong>on</strong>gruent shapes being present at <strong>the</strong><br />

time <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> word presentati<strong>on</strong>, but this was primarily true at short<br />

SOAs (100 ms or less). They took this interference to be visual,<br />

unlike <strong>the</strong> verbal interference in <strong>the</strong> standard <strong>Stroop</strong> task.<br />

MacLe~d and Dunbar (1988) assigned color names to random<br />

THE STROOP EFFECT 169<br />

polyg<strong>on</strong>s, and trained subjects in giving <strong>the</strong> new color name to<br />

each shape, m<strong>on</strong>itoring <strong>the</strong> development <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interference with<br />

practice. The advantage <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> studies using shapes is that <strong>the</strong>y step<br />

free <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words and reading, which dominate this literature.<br />

Few studies have used naturally colored objects, although<br />

Cramer (1967) and Arochova (1971) did develop an object-<br />

based test for use with children. When Mtnard-Buteau and<br />

Cavanagh (1984) had subjects name <strong>the</strong> color <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> an inc<strong>on</strong>gru-<br />

ously colored object, such as a blue banana, as opposed to a<br />

neutral object, such as a blue book, <strong>the</strong>y found interference.<br />

However, practice in <strong>the</strong> word versi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> same task did not<br />

transfer to <strong>the</strong> object versi<strong>on</strong>, so <strong>the</strong>y took <strong>the</strong>se to be indepen-<br />

dent types <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interference, <strong>the</strong> object-related <strong>on</strong>e being due to a<br />

more perceptual process. This also relates to <strong>the</strong> semantic ma-<br />

nipulati<strong>on</strong>s (discussed under Semantic Variati<strong>on</strong>) and to <strong>the</strong> re-<br />

lati<strong>on</strong> between <strong>the</strong> picture-word and color-word tasks.<br />

Arrows have been moderately popular stimuli. In <strong>the</strong> first<br />

such study, Shot (1970,1971) embedded <strong>the</strong> words left, right, up,<br />

and down in arrows pointing in directi<strong>on</strong>s o<strong>the</strong>r than <strong>the</strong> <strong>on</strong>e<br />

named by <strong>the</strong> word. The c<strong>on</strong>trol c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> omitted <strong>the</strong> words.<br />

Shor observed interference in naming <strong>the</strong> directi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong><br />

arrow, but smaller in magnitude than comm<strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong> color-<br />

word task. This interference decreased as <strong>the</strong> semantic overlap<br />

between <strong>the</strong> embedded word and <strong>the</strong> arrow declined (Fox,<br />

Shor, & Steinman, 1971). Shot, Hatch, Huds<strong>on</strong>, Landrigan, and<br />

Shaffer (1972) dem<strong>on</strong>strated that extensive training (30-50<br />

days) in naming such inc<strong>on</strong>gruent stimuli did not eliminate <strong>the</strong><br />

interference observed.<br />

Shor's task is analogous to <strong>the</strong> picture-word task in that <strong>the</strong><br />

two dimensi<strong>on</strong>s are not integrated, unlike <strong>the</strong> standard <strong>Stroop</strong><br />

task. To integrate <strong>the</strong> two dimensi<strong>on</strong>s more, Dyer (1972) used a<br />

moving display in which <strong>the</strong> same four directi<strong>on</strong>al words (or <strong>the</strong><br />

c<strong>on</strong>trol stimulus XXXX) could be moved ei<strong>the</strong>r in <strong>the</strong> named<br />

directi<strong>on</strong> (c<strong>on</strong>gruent) or in ano<strong>the</strong>r directi<strong>on</strong> (inc<strong>on</strong>gruent). He<br />

observed both interference and facilitati<strong>on</strong> for naming direc-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong>, but <strong>the</strong> interference was again small relative to <strong>the</strong> stan-<br />

dard <strong>Stroop</strong> task. Two o<strong>the</strong>r similar studies produced <strong>the</strong> same<br />

results. Clark and Brownell (1975) had subjects judge whe<strong>the</strong>r<br />

an arrow was pointing up or down inside a rectangle; White<br />

(1969) used <strong>the</strong> words north, south, east, and west inside a rect-<br />

angle, and had subjects name <strong>the</strong> word's positi<strong>on</strong>. Both studies<br />

reported small but reliable interference.<br />

A related series <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> studies began with <strong>the</strong> work <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Seymour<br />

(1973, 1974). He presented <strong>the</strong> words above, below, left, and<br />

right, and required subjects to name <strong>the</strong> locati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> word<br />

relative to a dot. Subjects showed both facilitati<strong>on</strong> and interfer-<br />

ence. Palefand Ols<strong>on</strong> (1975) followed up this work, presenting<br />

<strong>the</strong> words above and below ei<strong>the</strong>r above or below <strong>the</strong> fixati<strong>on</strong><br />

point. When spatial locati<strong>on</strong> was processed faster, resp<strong>on</strong>ses to<br />

word meaning showed interference from inc<strong>on</strong>gruent loca-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong>s; when word meaning was processed faster, resp<strong>on</strong>ses to<br />

spatial locati<strong>on</strong> showed interference from inc<strong>on</strong>gruent words.<br />

This has become a hallmark finding in support <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> idea that<br />

interference is a direct c<strong>on</strong>sequence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> speed <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing<br />

each dimensi<strong>on</strong>. Palef (1978) <strong>the</strong>n complicated <strong>the</strong> stimulus,<br />

adding an asterisk so that subjects could resp<strong>on</strong>d to relative or<br />

absolute positi<strong>on</strong> as well as to <strong>the</strong> word itself. She argued that<br />

<strong>the</strong> effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> two potentially interfering dimensi<strong>on</strong>s were<br />

additive. Logan and Zbrod<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>f(1979; see also Logan, 1980) ex-


170 COLIN M. MACLEOD<br />

tended this work by manipulating <strong>the</strong> frequency <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> trial types<br />

(see Probability <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Various Trial Types). More recently, Harvey<br />

(1984) examined SOA effects using <strong>the</strong> words hi and lo in com-<br />

patible versus incompatible positi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

<strong>An</strong>o<strong>the</strong>r perennial favorite is numerosity. Windes (1968) first<br />

showed that counting is interfered with if <strong>the</strong> stimuli being<br />

counted are incompatible numerals. Shor (1971) and Fox et al.<br />

(1971) reported similar interference effects with numbers. Mor-<br />

t<strong>on</strong> (1969) also obtained reliable interference from <strong>the</strong> presence<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> irrelevant digits in counting small sets <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> stimuli. In a more<br />

complicated study, Flowers, Warner, and Polansky (1979) dem-<br />

<strong>on</strong>strated that <strong>the</strong>re was also a major c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

modality to <strong>the</strong> likelihood <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> observing interference in a numer-<br />

osity task.<br />

Since Nav<strong>on</strong>'s (1977) and Stifling and Col<strong>the</strong>art's (1977) arti-<br />

cles <strong>on</strong> global versus local features in percepti<strong>on</strong>, this task has<br />

become popular as well. Here subjects saw a large letter com-<br />

posed <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> small letters (e.g., <strong>the</strong> letter H formed from small Hs vs.<br />

small Ss). When required to resp<strong>on</strong>d to <strong>the</strong> large letter, <strong>the</strong><br />

small <strong>on</strong>es had little impact. When required to resp<strong>on</strong>d to <strong>the</strong><br />

small letters, <strong>the</strong> large <strong>on</strong>e interfered. A similar effect was ob-<br />

tained using shapes: The global feature again overrode <strong>the</strong> local<br />

feature. Later studies (e.g., Kinchla, Solis-Macias, & H<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>fman,<br />

1983) pursued <strong>the</strong> role <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> attenti<strong>on</strong> in this remarkably good<br />

<strong>Stroop</strong> analog. Martin (1979) suggested that global features<br />

dominate for a multiple-element display, whereas local features<br />

take over for a few-element display.<br />

Egeth, Blecker, and Kamlet (1969) found no interference<br />

when subjects indicated whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> word and its ink color<br />

matched using <strong>the</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>ses "same" and "different" but reli-<br />

able interference when <strong>the</strong> words were switched to <strong>the</strong> stimuli<br />

same and d/ft. Thus, words can get through even in a compari-<br />

s<strong>on</strong> task, but <strong>the</strong>y must relate to <strong>the</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>se. Francolini and<br />

Egeth (1980) found <strong>Stroop</strong>-like interference <strong>on</strong>ly when manipu-<br />

lati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> relevant, to-be-named stimuli were undertaken in<br />

a color-numerosity task. Virzi and Egeth (1984) used a versi<strong>on</strong><br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> task in <strong>the</strong> perceptual domain to examine <strong>the</strong><br />

phenomen<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> illusory c<strong>on</strong>juncti<strong>on</strong>s, finding that subjects<br />

have difficulty discriminating whe<strong>the</strong>r informati<strong>on</strong> came from<br />

a color patch or a word.<br />

Daniel (1968, 1969, 1970a, 1970b) tried several variati<strong>on</strong>s to<br />

heighten interference in <strong>the</strong> standard color-word task. He<br />

printed <strong>the</strong> color words in color and added a differently colored<br />

background, or he had a tape recorder spewing out color words<br />

while subjects attempted to name <strong>the</strong> ink colors <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> inc<strong>on</strong>gruent<br />

words. Interference increased, but overall resp<strong>on</strong>se rate also<br />

slowed down. O<strong>the</strong>rs also altered <strong>the</strong> standard color-word task<br />

(e.g., Dyer, 1973b; Friedman & Derks, 1973; Hall & Swane, 1973;<br />

Uleman & Reeves, 1971). Sometimes more than <strong>on</strong>e <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong><br />

dimensi<strong>on</strong>s have been combined, as in <strong>the</strong> case <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> color-<br />

digit interference task devised by Wolitzky, H<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>er, and Shapiro<br />

(1972).<br />

The recent past has seen extensi<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> effect even<br />

far<strong>the</strong>r afield to domains such as affect (Gardner, 1985) and<br />

even arachnophobia (Watts, McKenna, Sharrock, & Trezise,<br />

1986). How equivalent are all <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>se tasks that superficially<br />

resemble <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> task? Even for <strong>the</strong> very prevalent alterna-<br />

tives, such as <strong>the</strong> picture-word task, we do not know. Correla-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong>al studies are <strong>on</strong>e way to find out, but few have been per-<br />

formed. Obviously, though, it is <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>oretical importance to<br />

know whe<strong>the</strong>r similar processes are invoked in <strong>the</strong>se many vari-<br />

ati<strong>on</strong>s, but we have insufficient evidence at present.<br />

From here <strong>on</strong>, <strong>the</strong> secti<strong>on</strong>s end with empirical generaliza-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong>s where warranted. These are collected in Appendix B to<br />

form <strong>the</strong> basic findings that must be accommodated by any<br />

viable <strong>the</strong>ory <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> effect. Then, under Theoretical Ac-<br />

counts <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> <strong>Effect</strong>, <strong>the</strong> existing <strong>the</strong>ories are c<strong>on</strong>sidered<br />

in view <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> this fundamental evidence. Here, <strong>the</strong>n, is <strong>the</strong> first<br />

such summary statement: The <strong>Stroop</strong> effect is observed with lists<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> stimuli, with single stimuli, and with many variati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se required. Similar data patterns are evident in numerous<br />

<strong>Stroop</strong> analogs, such as <strong>the</strong> picture-word task. I now c<strong>on</strong>sider<br />

what has been learned about <strong>the</strong> causes <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>Stroop</strong> interference<br />

from <strong>the</strong> hundreds <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> studies over <strong>the</strong> past five decades.<br />

Manipulati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Informati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> Critical Trials<br />

From here <strong>on</strong>, empirical studies are grouped as to whe<strong>the</strong>r<br />

<strong>the</strong>ir manipulati<strong>on</strong>s pertain to individual trials, <strong>the</strong> overall ap-<br />

proach to <strong>the</strong> whole task, <strong>the</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>se demands, or qualities <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

<strong>the</strong> individual subject. Such divisi<strong>on</strong>s are comm<strong>on</strong> in cognitive<br />

psychology and serve a useful organizati<strong>on</strong>al functi<strong>on</strong> here.<br />

Hue Variati<strong>on</strong><br />

Most manipulati<strong>on</strong>s in <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> task have centered <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

to-be-ignored verbal dimensi<strong>on</strong>. Only a few have examined <strong>the</strong><br />

hues <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> to-be-named inks (e.g., Sichel & Chandler, 1969).<br />

Dyer (197 lb) showed that subjects named achromatic color<br />

patches slightly faster than chromatic patches. When <strong>the</strong><br />

patches were replaced with inc<strong>on</strong>gruent color words, interfer-<br />

ence was greater for <strong>the</strong> chromatic than for <strong>the</strong> achromatic ver-<br />

si<strong>on</strong>. Dyer argued that interference enhanced basic processing-<br />

time differences.<br />

The <strong>on</strong>ly o<strong>the</strong>r approach to varying hue was that <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Flowers<br />

and his colleagues (Flowers & Blair, 1976; Flowers & Dutch,<br />

1976). Flowers and Blair had subjects classify six colors into two<br />

categories, and found that a "natural" split <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> adjacent hues<br />

(red, orange, and yellow vs. green, blue, and purple) caused<br />

interference <strong>on</strong>ly if blocks <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> n<strong>on</strong>adjacent hues were included<br />

(red, yellow, and blue vs. orange, green, and purple). However,<br />

interference was always present when using <strong>the</strong> n<strong>on</strong>adjacent<br />

classificati<strong>on</strong>. Flowers and Dutch replicated this, and argued<br />

that n<strong>on</strong>adjacent categorizati<strong>on</strong> required access to names, not<br />

just to perceptual qualities <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> colors, perhaps because <strong>the</strong><br />

n<strong>on</strong>natural set members had to be c<strong>on</strong>stantly rehearsed.<br />

Most <strong>Stroop</strong> studies use from two to six hues, usually highly<br />

differentiable, prototypical colors. Yet variati<strong>on</strong>s in number,<br />

saturati<strong>on</strong>, similarity, or o<strong>the</strong>r features <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> color dimensi<strong>on</strong><br />

could be used to manipulate processing <strong>on</strong> that dimensi<strong>on</strong>, and<br />

would be relevant to <strong>the</strong> explanati<strong>on</strong>s discussed under Theoreti-<br />

cal Accounts <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> <strong>Effect</strong>. With so few relevant experi-<br />

ments, however, it is premature to <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>fer an empirical generaliza-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong> now.<br />

Acoustic Variati<strong>on</strong><br />

A comm<strong>on</strong> introspecti<strong>on</strong> attributes interference to <strong>the</strong> artic-<br />

ulatory-ph<strong>on</strong>emic-acoustic dimensi<strong>on</strong>. Before it can be pre-


vented, <strong>the</strong> wr<strong>on</strong>g resp<strong>on</strong>se is begun, and its sound c<strong>on</strong>flicts<br />

with producing <strong>the</strong> right resp<strong>on</strong>se. Hollingworth (1915) sug-<br />

gested a versi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> this idea, which is quite c<strong>on</strong>sistent with <strong>the</strong><br />

hypo<strong>the</strong>sis that <strong>Stroop</strong> interference occurs at a resp<strong>on</strong>se stage<br />

late in processing (e.g., Posner & Snyder, 1975) after at least part<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> word has been identified.<br />

Bakan and Alpers<strong>on</strong> (1967; see also Langer & Rosenberg,<br />

1966) found increasing interference with increasing pro-<br />

nounceability <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> n<strong>on</strong>sense syllables in <strong>the</strong> color-word task.<br />

However, pr<strong>on</strong>ounceability was not <strong>the</strong> whole story: Words<br />

equated with n<strong>on</strong>sense syllables for pr<strong>on</strong>ounceability produced<br />

still more interference. Thus, meaning also matters, as dis-<br />

cussed under Semantic Variati<strong>on</strong>. Still <strong>the</strong>ir study suggested<br />

some role for n<strong>on</strong>semantic factors.<br />

Shortly afterward, Dalrymple-Alford (1972a, 1972b) pursued<br />

pr<strong>on</strong>unciati<strong>on</strong> effects. In additi<strong>on</strong> to color words, he used three<br />

types <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> n<strong>on</strong>color words: same initial sound (e.g., run for redand<br />

grown for green), same terminal sound (e.g., Jed for red and clean<br />

for green), or unrelated acoustically (e.g., cat for red and flown<br />

for green, an unfortunate choice given <strong>the</strong> comm<strong>on</strong> final pho-<br />

neme). Color words caused <strong>the</strong> most interference (177 ms), but<br />

both types <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> acoustic relati<strong>on</strong>--initial sound (62 ms) and termi-<br />

nal sound (49 ms)--also caused interference. Dalrymple-Alford<br />

proposed that a competing color name could be primed seman-<br />

tically (1972a) or acoustically (1972b).<br />

Singer, Lappin, and Moore 0975) c<strong>on</strong>trasted <strong>the</strong> effect <strong>on</strong><br />

ink-color naming <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> first, middle, or last two letters <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a<br />

color word versus <strong>the</strong> full color word. First letters interfered<br />

more than later letters, but not as much as <strong>the</strong> whole word.<br />

Adding unrelated letters to <strong>the</strong> first letters did not alter interfer-<br />

ence, leading Singer et al. to speculate that <strong>the</strong> interference<br />

stemmed from activati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a motor program to articulate <strong>the</strong><br />

beginning <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a word. Regan (1978) c<strong>on</strong>firmed this initial-letter<br />

effect, also showing that <strong>the</strong> first letter could produce facilita-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>gruent case. She took this as evidence for auto-<br />

matic processing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> letters and words.<br />

Underwood, Briggs, and Underwood (1984) manipulated <strong>the</strong><br />

ph<strong>on</strong>emic or graphemic relati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a word or a n<strong>on</strong>word to <strong>the</strong><br />

ink-color name. For both good and poor 9- to I l-year-old<br />

readers, c<strong>on</strong>gruent items generated facilitati<strong>on</strong> and inc<strong>on</strong>-<br />

gruent items generated interference. So far all <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>se studies<br />

show at least an orthographic/graphemic effect: Comm<strong>on</strong> let-<br />

ters, especially beginning a word, have an influence. However,<br />

it is harder to tell whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong>re was any acoustic/ph<strong>on</strong>emic<br />

effect (with <strong>the</strong> possible excepti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> Bakan and Alpers<strong>on</strong><br />

study).<br />

A study by Dennis and Newstead (1981) dem<strong>on</strong>strated that<br />

acoustic/ph<strong>on</strong>emic effects are indeed present. They compared<br />

color words with pseudohomoph<strong>on</strong>es (e.g., blue vs, bloo) and<br />

found similar interference. Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, this was not due just<br />

to <strong>the</strong>ir comm<strong>on</strong> initial letters because <strong>the</strong> baseline was a n<strong>on</strong>-<br />

word matched for visual similarity and sharing <strong>the</strong> initial letters<br />

(e.g., blir). Interestingly, though, real color words led to facilita-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>gruent trials but pseudohomoph<strong>on</strong>es did not. This<br />

is just <strong>on</strong>e <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> many situati<strong>on</strong>s where facilitati<strong>on</strong> and interfer-<br />

ence come decoupled.<br />

In presenting a prime word before a <strong>Stroop</strong> trial, Tanenhaus,<br />

Flanigan, and Seidenberg 0980) found that if <strong>the</strong> prime was<br />

related orthographically or ph<strong>on</strong>ologically, or both, to <strong>the</strong> irrele-<br />

THE STROOP EFFECT 171<br />

vant word <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> trial, interference with color naming<br />

was greater than if <strong>the</strong> prime and irrelevant words were unre-<br />

lated. This occurred for both auditorily and visually presented<br />

primes, generalizing <strong>the</strong> ph<strong>on</strong>ological effect in <strong>Stroop</strong> interfer-<br />

ence.<br />

What about facilitati<strong>on</strong>? Effler (1978b; see also 1977a) re-<br />

ported reduced interference when <strong>the</strong> irrelevant word was<br />

acoustically similar to its to-be-named ink color. The effect also<br />

obtained when <strong>the</strong> relati<strong>on</strong> was between <strong>the</strong> ink color <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> item n<br />

and <strong>the</strong> word in item n + 1, but not vice versa. Apparently,<br />

having both dimensi<strong>on</strong>s c<strong>on</strong>verge <strong>on</strong> a comm<strong>on</strong>-sounding<br />

name speeds resp<strong>on</strong>ding. Sequential effects like <strong>the</strong>se are dis-<br />

cussed more extensively under Trial Sequence.<br />

Four studies have tried to tie up <strong>the</strong> articulatory system to<br />

determine what c<strong>on</strong>sequences this would have in <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong><br />

task. All used <strong>the</strong> card-sorting paradigm. Martin (1978) re-<br />

ported reduced interference in sorting by color when subjects<br />

had to say "blab, blah" c<strong>on</strong>currently. However, Besner, Davies,<br />

and Daniels (1981) reported no reducti<strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong> same task.<br />

Martin (1981) found that simuRaneous irrelevant articulati<strong>on</strong><br />

had no effect <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> reverse <strong>Stroop</strong> interference she observed.<br />

Chmiel (1984) c<strong>on</strong>firmed all three <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>se results, implicating<br />

<strong>the</strong> labeling <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> sorting bins as critical. When labeled with<br />

color patches, interference was reduced during articulatory<br />

suppressi<strong>on</strong>; when labeled with words, <strong>the</strong>re was no reducti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Using <strong>the</strong> "large letter composed <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> little letters" task, Stifling<br />

and Col<strong>the</strong>art (1977) argued against an acoustic effect. Making<br />

<strong>the</strong> small letters easier to c<strong>on</strong>fuse visually with <strong>the</strong> large letter<br />

increased interference, but making <strong>the</strong>m easier to c<strong>on</strong>fuse<br />

acoustically had no impact. Possibly, this result reflects differ-<br />

ent processes in <strong>the</strong> two tasks. For example, letter-shape analy-<br />

sis seems to be more critical in this task than in <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> task.<br />

Investigati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> resp<strong>on</strong>se modality (e.g., vocal vs. butt<strong>on</strong>press)<br />

might help elucidate such task differences.<br />

In <strong>the</strong> picture-word task, Ehri (1977) dem<strong>on</strong>strated that<br />

nouns interfered more than adjectives or functors. Ehri took<br />

this to mean that interference was not ph<strong>on</strong>ologically based, or<br />

all word types should have caused similar interference. How-<br />

ever, using n<strong>on</strong>sense syllables as a baseline does present prob-<br />

lems (cf. Bakan & Alpers<strong>on</strong>, 1967), as Ehri acknowledged.<br />

Lupker 0982) obtained results more like Effler's (1978b): Or-<br />

thographic and ph<strong>on</strong>emic similarity between <strong>the</strong> word and <strong>the</strong><br />

picture's name led to facilitati<strong>on</strong> relative to a n<strong>on</strong>word (but not<br />

to a picture al<strong>on</strong>e). Lupker attributed this benefit to decreased<br />

name-retrieval time for <strong>the</strong> picture, an effect he saw as separa-<br />

ble from <strong>the</strong> basic resp<strong>on</strong>se-competiti<strong>on</strong> source <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interference.<br />

At least for <strong>the</strong> standard color-word task, <strong>the</strong>n, <strong>the</strong> visual or<br />

acoustic characteristics, or both, <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> irrelevant word appear<br />

to exert an influence. Much <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> this hinges <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> compatibility<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> ink name and <strong>the</strong> first letter or two <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> word, although<br />

later parts <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> word can also have some effect. To summarize,<br />

both orthographic and particularly acoustic/articulatory relati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

between <strong>the</strong> irrelevant word (or part <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> word) and <strong>the</strong> to-be-<br />

named ink color c<strong>on</strong>tribute to <strong>the</strong> interference.<br />

As a final note, <strong>on</strong>e useful study would combine <strong>the</strong> articula-<br />

tory-suppressi<strong>on</strong> technique <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Martin (1978, 1981), Chmiel<br />

(1984), and Besner et al. (1981) with <strong>the</strong> letter manipulati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

Dalrymple-Alford (1972b) and Singer et al. (1975). Would in-<br />

compatible letters interfere as much if <strong>the</strong>y could not be articu-


172 COLIN M. MACLEOD<br />

lated? Regardless, n<strong>on</strong>semantic features <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> words do c<strong>on</strong>trib-<br />

ute to interference, supporting <strong>the</strong> intuiti<strong>on</strong>s--and <strong>the</strong> articula-<br />

tory stumbles--<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> subjects in <strong>Stroop</strong> experiments.<br />

Semantic Variati<strong>on</strong><br />

Since Klein's (1964) benchmark study, how word meaning<br />

influences color naming has been attacked from a host <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> dif-<br />

ferent angles. Indeed, <strong>the</strong> enterprise has come full circle: The<br />

<strong>Stroop</strong> task has become a comm<strong>on</strong> tool in understanding how<br />

we draw meaning from words. In this secti<strong>on</strong>, I begin with<br />

studies manipulating <strong>the</strong> meaning <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a single irrelevant word,<br />

and <strong>the</strong>n turn to studies involving priming with words or<br />

phrases.<br />

Semantic variati<strong>on</strong> and <strong>the</strong> irrelevant word. The classic study<br />

is that <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Klein (1964, Experiment 1). His goal was to under-<br />

stand <strong>the</strong> sources <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> word's interfering effect in color nam-<br />

ing and <strong>the</strong> processes involved in that interference. To do this,<br />

he manipulated <strong>the</strong> relati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> four words to four ink colors<br />

(red, green, yellow, and blue). All subjects performed <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>-<br />

trol, "colors-al<strong>on</strong>e" card, averaging about 44 s for <strong>the</strong> card, and<br />

<strong>the</strong>n performed <strong>on</strong>e <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> six interference cards. As always, <strong>the</strong><br />

standard inc<strong>on</strong>gruent c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> (where red, green, yellow, and<br />

blue are <strong>the</strong> words) showed large interference: an increase <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

about 37.5 s. When four o<strong>the</strong>r color words were used (tan, pur-<br />

ple, gray, and black), interference was cut to 18 s. Interference<br />

decreased fur<strong>the</strong>r to 15.5 s for color-related words (fire, grass,<br />

lem<strong>on</strong>, and sky). Substituting comm<strong>on</strong>, unassociated words<br />

(put, heart, take, and friend) or rare words (sol, helot, eft, and<br />

abjure) dropped interference to 12 s and 7.5 s, respectively. Fi-<br />

nally, unpr<strong>on</strong>ounceable, n<strong>on</strong>sense syllables (hjh, evgjc, bhdr,<br />

and gsxrq) produced <strong>on</strong>ly 5 s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interference.<br />

Although some <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Klein's items were not ideal (e.g., bhdr,<br />

gsxrq, and especially grass, which share initial letters with target<br />

color names), his basic point was sound: The more meaningful<br />

<strong>the</strong> irrelevant word, <strong>the</strong> more interference it caused. This was<br />

particularly true for color-related words, but even n<strong>on</strong>color<br />

words produced some interference. Klein's experiment opened<br />

<strong>the</strong> floodgates for numerous studies <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> how word meaning af-<br />

fects color naming.<br />

Dalrymple-Alford (1968, 1972a; Dalrymple-Alford & Az-<br />

koul, 1972; see also Harris<strong>on</strong> & Boese, 1976) extended Klein's<br />

procedure. Dalrymple-Alford (1972a) found that, compared<br />

with unrelated c<strong>on</strong>trol words, color words interfered more than<br />

color-related words (red, 131 ms vs. blood, 43 ms), but both<br />

caused reliable interference, replicating Klein. More novel,<br />

both also caused reliable facilitati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>gruent trials (red, 63<br />

ms, and blood, 42 ms). Dalrymple-Alford discounted a purely<br />

articulatory explanati<strong>on</strong> because words like sky and snow (for<br />

blue and white) caused just as large effects as words like grass<br />

(for green) even without shared letters.<br />

As <strong>Stroop</strong> knew, choosing <strong>the</strong> appropriate c<strong>on</strong>trol stimulus is<br />

not trivial and warrants an aside here. Some studies use an<br />

unrelated word, some a n<strong>on</strong>word, some a row <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Xs or o<strong>the</strong>r<br />

redundant character, and some a color patch. For example,<br />

Dalrymple-Alford's (1972a) study also included a XXXX c<strong>on</strong>-<br />

trol. If XXXX ra<strong>the</strong>r than <strong>the</strong> unrelated word is used as <strong>the</strong><br />

baseline, interference estimates for <strong>the</strong> inc<strong>on</strong>gruent cases in-<br />

crease by 65 ms, and <strong>the</strong>re is no facilitati<strong>on</strong> for <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>gruent<br />

cases. A n<strong>on</strong>word c<strong>on</strong>trol might have produced yet ano<strong>the</strong>r<br />

baseline value.<br />

In general, using any characters in <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>trol c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong><br />

stands to be distracting and to increase <strong>the</strong> time to resp<strong>on</strong>d to<br />

<strong>the</strong> color, but substantial changes in interference typically re-<br />

quire some c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong> to <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>cept <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> color (or some kind <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

priming). Thus, although Hintzman et al. (1972) obtained reli-<br />

able interference and facilitati<strong>on</strong> for color words relative to a<br />

n<strong>on</strong>word c<strong>on</strong>trol, words unrelated to color did not differ from<br />

<strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>trol. A similar result was reported by Redding and Get-<br />

jets (1977), who also found that scrambled color words were<br />

equivalent to n<strong>on</strong>color words. Using unrelated words versus<br />

n<strong>on</strong>words, <strong>the</strong>n, makes little difference, hut <strong>the</strong> switch to color<br />

patches or XXXX-type c<strong>on</strong>trols may have more effect.<br />

Scheibe, Shaver, and Carrier (1967) found that amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

interference in color naming was a direct functi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

color associati<strong>on</strong> for <strong>the</strong> irrelevant words, and argued that word<br />

frequency was critical. 5 Langlois (1974) reported that a direct<br />

manipulati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> word frequency influenced amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interfer-<br />

ence, hut he failed to separate resp<strong>on</strong>se-set membership from<br />

<strong>the</strong> frequency manipulati<strong>on</strong>, a comm<strong>on</strong> problem in <strong>Stroop</strong> re-<br />

search. Effier (1977a, 1980, 1981) has also reported frequency<br />

effects.<br />

Proctor (1978) pointed out that strength <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> associati<strong>on</strong> and<br />

frequency were typically c<strong>on</strong>founded with resp<strong>on</strong>se-set mem-<br />

bership. He broke this c<strong>on</strong>founding and showed that resp<strong>on</strong>se-<br />

set members still interfered more than n<strong>on</strong>members. For color<br />

words not in <strong>the</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>se set, interference was a direct functi<strong>on</strong><br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> strength <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> associati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> words to <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>cept <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

color, a c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong> in accord with that <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Fox et al. (1971).<br />

Alpers<strong>on</strong> (1967) trained subjects <strong>on</strong> paired associates where<br />

<strong>the</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>se terms were ei<strong>the</strong>r c<strong>on</strong>nected or unc<strong>on</strong>nected to<br />

<strong>the</strong> color-naming task. When <strong>the</strong> stimulus terms were made <strong>the</strong><br />

irrelevant materials in <strong>the</strong> color-naming task, semantic related-<br />

ness <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>se terms had a c<strong>on</strong>siderable effect, even with<br />

<strong>the</strong>se newly learned pairs. To bolster <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong> that mean-<br />

ing was <strong>the</strong> key, Bakan and Alpers<strong>on</strong> (1967) dem<strong>on</strong>strated that<br />

words interfered more than did n<strong>on</strong>sense syllables equated for<br />

pr<strong>on</strong>ounceability.<br />

Using <strong>the</strong> same approach, Pritchatt (1968) had subjects asso-<br />

ciate n<strong>on</strong>sense syllables with colors, and observed interference<br />

when <strong>the</strong> n<strong>on</strong>sense syllables appeared later as <strong>the</strong> irrelevant<br />

dimensi<strong>on</strong> in a color-naming task. <strong>An</strong>alogously, Stirling (1979)<br />

required subjects to learn to resp<strong>on</strong>d to color patches with letter<br />

names. When <strong>the</strong>se letters became <strong>the</strong> inc<strong>on</strong>gruent dimensi<strong>on</strong><br />

in color naming, interference resulted. Similar procedures have<br />

been used elsewhere (e.g., Glaser & Dolt, 1977; MacLeod &<br />

Dunbar, 1988). Of course, whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong>se studies speak to <strong>the</strong><br />

role <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> meaning in interference depends up<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> representa-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong> underlying such paired-associate learning.<br />

More in keeping with Klein's procedure, White (1969) had<br />

subjects identify color versus directi<strong>on</strong> attributes in <strong>the</strong> pres-<br />

ence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> inc<strong>on</strong>gruent attribute names. The more semantically<br />

similar <strong>the</strong> inc<strong>on</strong>gruent attribute name, <strong>the</strong> more interference<br />

5 A set <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> norms for colors and color words has since been c<strong>on</strong>-<br />

structed and could be used in a detailed investigati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> associati<strong>on</strong><br />

(Solso, 1971).


was obtained, particularly for <strong>the</strong> color dimensi<strong>on</strong>. Fox et al.<br />

(1971) showed semantic gradients <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interference <strong>on</strong> several<br />

<strong>Stroop</strong> analogs, including spatial directi<strong>on</strong> and numerosity, and<br />

str<strong>on</strong>gly supported Klein's arguments.<br />

Murray, Mastr<strong>on</strong>ardi, and Duncan (1972) also supported<br />

Klein in showing that subjects named <strong>the</strong> colors <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> color words<br />

more slowly than those <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> animal words. However, when sub-<br />

jects were required to sort items <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> basis <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong><br />

words or <strong>the</strong> ink colors were <strong>the</strong> same or different, colors were<br />

sorted faster than words. The nature <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> task can completely<br />

determine <strong>the</strong> form <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> interference observed. Similar points<br />

have been made in <strong>the</strong> picture-word work (Glaser & Glaser,<br />

1989; Smith & Magee, 1980) and in <strong>the</strong> priming work (Henik et<br />

al, 1983).<br />

In an <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>ten-cited study, Keele 0972) argued that interference<br />

must occur after <strong>the</strong> memory-retrieval stage. When subjects<br />

pressed butt<strong>on</strong>s to indicate <strong>the</strong>ir resp<strong>on</strong>ses (ra<strong>the</strong>r than speak-<br />

ing <strong>the</strong>m), <strong>on</strong>ly color words interfered; n<strong>on</strong>color words and<br />

scrambled color words did not differ from his c<strong>on</strong>trol (letter-<br />

like Gibs<strong>on</strong> forms). The scrambled-c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> result is surpris-<br />

ing, though; every stimulus began with <strong>the</strong> same letter as its<br />

corresp<strong>on</strong>ding word (rde, gnere, ywoloe, and belu), which ordi-<br />

narily causes interference (see Acoustic Variati<strong>on</strong>). 6 Possibly<br />

Keele's use <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> manual resp<strong>on</strong>ses, known to decrease interfer-<br />

ence overall, is resp<strong>on</strong>sible (see Resp<strong>on</strong>se Modality: Oral Versus<br />

Manual).<br />

Many investigators have manipulated <strong>the</strong> meaning <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> sin-<br />

gle, irrelevant word. For instance, Etiler (1978a) practiced sub-<br />

jects <strong>on</strong> color-related versus color-unrelated words and re-<br />

ported a decreased difference in interference with practice.<br />

Also Ridley, Johns<strong>on</strong>, and Braisted (1978; see also Ridley, 1980)<br />

presented evidence that semantic-differential ratings <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> n<strong>on</strong>-<br />

color words predicted color-naming times. In sum, <strong>the</strong> many<br />

manipulati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> meaning <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> irrelevant word <strong>on</strong> a single<br />

<strong>Stroop</strong> trial all c<strong>on</strong>verge <strong>on</strong> this empirical generalizati<strong>on</strong>: Com-<br />

pared with naming <strong>the</strong> ink color al<strong>on</strong>e, irrelevant verbal stimuli<br />

unrelated to <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>cept <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> color inteoCere <strong>on</strong>ly minimally with<br />

color naming. However, as <strong>the</strong> words semantic associati<strong>on</strong> to <strong>the</strong><br />

c<strong>on</strong>cept <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> color increases, so does its potential to interfere.<br />

Semantic variati<strong>on</strong> induced by priming. Since <strong>the</strong> early<br />

1970s, <strong>the</strong> semantic-priming technique has had increasing c<strong>on</strong>-<br />

tact with <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> literature. Probably <strong>the</strong> two most widely<br />

known members <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> this set are <strong>the</strong> dissertati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Warren<br />

0972; see also Warren 1974) and C<strong>on</strong>rad (1974). Discussing<br />

<strong>the</strong>se in detail will both introduce <strong>the</strong> method and set <strong>the</strong> stage<br />

for <strong>the</strong> variati<strong>on</strong>s that have succeeded <strong>the</strong>m.<br />

Warrens (1972) basic idea was that <strong>the</strong> irrelevant word <strong>on</strong> a<br />

<strong>Stroop</strong> trial should interfere more if it had just been activated<br />

by a preceding event. Preceding each critical color-word trial,<br />

he placed a priming event. On c<strong>on</strong>trol trials, this event c<strong>on</strong>-<br />

sisted <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> phrase "No list this trial:' On priming trials, <strong>the</strong><br />

event c<strong>on</strong>sisted <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a list <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> three words from a single semantic<br />

category: aunt, uncle, cousin or dog, cat, horse, for example.<br />

Then <strong>the</strong> critical color-naming trial occurred, with ei<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong><br />

category name (relatives), a repeated category member (aunt), an<br />

unrelated word (doctor), or a row <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Xs as <strong>the</strong> "word y<br />

What Warren observed was increased interference in naming<br />

<strong>the</strong> ink color when <strong>the</strong> irrelevant word was primed. For <strong>the</strong><br />

words aunt or relatives in red ink, subjects were slower to say<br />

THE STROOP EFFECT 173<br />

"red" if <strong>the</strong>y had just seen relevant primes; <strong>the</strong> effect was some-<br />

what larger for aunt. In later experiments, Warren established<br />

that this activati<strong>on</strong> persisted over at least five intervening items<br />

when subjects had to remember earlier words for a final test,<br />

but <strong>on</strong>ly a bit over <strong>on</strong>e intervening item (30 s) when this require-<br />

ment was dropped. This was true whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> critical irrelevant<br />

word was <strong>the</strong> category name or a repeated category member. He<br />

did not test n<strong>on</strong>studied category members.<br />

In his 1974 follow-up study, Warren varied <strong>the</strong> associative<br />

strength between an auditory prime word and <strong>the</strong> irrelevant<br />

visual word <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> critical color-naming trial. The relati<strong>on</strong><br />

could be high (king-queen), medium (bath-clean), or low (wish-<br />

dream). Subtracting <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>trol times for unrelated auditory<br />

primes (e.g., food, law, or hand), <strong>the</strong>re was significant interfer-<br />

ence for high (95 ms) and medium (50 ms) but not for low (20<br />

ms) items. Thus, interference declined systematically with asso-<br />

ciative strength. Warren <strong>the</strong>n dem<strong>on</strong>strated that this occurred<br />

<strong>on</strong>ly for forward associates, not backward associates, str<strong>on</strong>gly<br />

suggesting that <strong>the</strong> prime activates <strong>the</strong> irrelevant word (ra<strong>the</strong>r<br />

than <strong>the</strong> reverse) <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> critical trial.<br />

C<strong>on</strong>rad's (1974) <strong>the</strong>sis used Warrens as a springboard, ex-<br />

tending <strong>the</strong> priming domain from words to sentences. When<br />

<strong>the</strong> sentence c<strong>on</strong>text came before <strong>the</strong> ambiguous word (e.g.,<br />

"The toy costs a nickel"), <strong>the</strong>re was c<strong>on</strong>siderable interference in<br />

color naming for <strong>the</strong> ambiguous word itself(nickel, 100 ms), <strong>the</strong><br />

appropriate category (m<strong>on</strong>ey, 106 ms), and even <strong>the</strong> inappropri-<br />

ate category (metal, 64 ms), relative to a c<strong>on</strong>trol sentence (e.g.,<br />

"The craftsman made <strong>the</strong> chair"). Apparently, both senses <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

nickel were activated by <strong>the</strong> sentence despite its clear meaning.<br />

Interestingly, when <strong>the</strong> sentence c<strong>on</strong>text came after <strong>the</strong> ambig-<br />

uous word (e.g., "That nickel is from her coin collecti<strong>on</strong>'), inter-<br />

ference in <strong>the</strong> three c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s virtually disappeared (19, 2, and<br />

35 ms, respectively). C<strong>on</strong>rad took this to indicate ei<strong>the</strong>r decay <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

activati<strong>on</strong> or interference from succeeding words. The critical<br />

result, though, was <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>text-before c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. She went <strong>on</strong> to<br />

show that <strong>the</strong> interfering capability <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> an ambiguous word was<br />

no greater in an ambiguous sentence: Both senses <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> word<br />

were activated regardless <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> how it was used in <strong>the</strong> sentence.<br />

Oden and Spira (1983) c<strong>on</strong>firmed that both senses <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> an ambigu-<br />

ous word were indeed activated, but that <strong>the</strong>ir extent <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> activa-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong> was affected by c<strong>on</strong>text. All candidates were activated, and<br />

<strong>the</strong>n fur<strong>the</strong>r processing narrowed <strong>the</strong> possibilities.<br />

Many studies sprang from those <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Warren and C<strong>on</strong>rad.<br />

McClain (1983a) attempted to link <strong>the</strong>ir logic with Klein's<br />

(1964). She manipulated both <strong>the</strong> prime word and <strong>the</strong> irrele-<br />

vant word <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> color-naming trial using inc<strong>on</strong>gruent combi-<br />

nati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong>ly. There were four critical relati<strong>on</strong>s: (a) prime-red,<br />

irrelevant-blood, (b) prime-apple, irrelevant-fire, (c) prime-ap-<br />

ple, irrelevant-apple, and (d) prime-nail, irrelevant-nail. Be-<br />

cause C<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> (a) explicitly activates color and C<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> (b)<br />

repeats <strong>the</strong> irrelevant but color-related word, she predicted that<br />

<strong>the</strong>se two c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s would produce more interference, which<br />

<strong>the</strong>y did.<br />

Merrill, Sperber, and McCauley (1981) pursued <strong>the</strong> influence<br />

6 Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, Michael Mass<strong>on</strong> and I have data showing that scram-<br />

bled color words with no letters in <strong>the</strong> correct positi<strong>on</strong> still cause inter-<br />

ference in an oral resp<strong>on</strong>se.


174 COLIN M. MACLEOD<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>text <strong>on</strong> semantic interpretati<strong>on</strong>. On a critical trial, a tar-<br />

get word such as claw appeared in a color to be named. Before<br />

<strong>the</strong> critical trial, ei<strong>the</strong>r an appropriate ("The girl fought <strong>the</strong><br />

cat"), an inappropriate ("The girl touched <strong>the</strong> cat"), or a neutral<br />

("The man fixed <strong>the</strong> car") sentence or word (cat vs. car) was<br />

displayed. Poor and good comprehenders showed equivalent<br />

interference when <strong>the</strong> prime was an appropriate or inappropri-<br />

ate word. However, when <strong>the</strong> prime was a sentence, poor com-<br />

prehenders again showed interference for both types <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> primes,<br />

but good comprehenders showed interference <strong>on</strong>ly for appro-<br />

priate primes. Merrill et al. c<strong>on</strong>cluded that good compre-<br />

henders encode words with reference to an entire sentence's<br />

meaning, whereas poor comprehenders encode <strong>the</strong> words as<br />

distinct units.<br />

Dosher and Corbett (1982) examined whe<strong>the</strong>r sentences like<br />

"The man swept <strong>the</strong> floor" activated relevant instruments (e.g.,<br />

broom). If so, <strong>the</strong>n broom should interfere with naming its color<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> print. There was no evidence for such activati<strong>on</strong>, relative to<br />

an unrelated prior sentence, except if subjects were explicitly<br />

told to think <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> instrument. Interestingly, such an instruc-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong> led to facilitati<strong>on</strong>, not interference, c<strong>on</strong>trary to <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r<br />

studies in this secti<strong>on</strong> (but see Keenan, Potts, Jennings, & Gold-<br />

ing, 1988, and Whitney, 1986, for similar findings). Perhaps ifa<br />

primed irrelevant word is processed especially quickly, it is<br />

dealt with before it can exert a negative effect. This idea merits<br />

fur<strong>the</strong>r explorati<strong>on</strong>; it may represent a potentially useful tool in<br />

psycholinguistic research.<br />

In a series <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> studies, Whitney and his colleagues (Whitney,<br />

1986; Whitney & Kellas, 1984; Whitney, McKay, Kellas, &<br />

Emers<strong>on</strong>, 1985) examined lexical access using <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> task.<br />

Initially, Whitney and KeUas (1984) presented sentences that<br />

biased a typical member <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a category (e.g., "The guest saw <strong>the</strong><br />

bird that landed <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> branch"--robin) or an atypical member<br />

(e.g., "The guest saw <strong>the</strong> bird that roasted <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> grill"--<br />

chicken), or that were neutral (e.g., "The guest saw <strong>the</strong> towels<br />

that hung in <strong>the</strong> bathroom"). Regardless <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> prime sentence,<br />

<strong>the</strong> typical word always caused interference, whereas <strong>the</strong> atypi-<br />

cal word did not. Yet typical words were better retrieval cues for<br />

typical sentences, and atypical words were better retrieval cues<br />

for atypical sentences. They c<strong>on</strong>cluded that category referents<br />

are not necessarily instantiated for single sentences.<br />

On <strong>the</strong> basis <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> manipulati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> item dominance and delay<br />

before testing, Whitney et al. (1985) c<strong>on</strong>firmed <strong>the</strong> findings <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

C<strong>on</strong>rad (1974) and <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Oden and Spira (1983) that initial seman-<br />

tic access is independent <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>text both for ambiguous and<br />

unambiguous nouns. Whitney's (1986) dissertati<strong>on</strong> extended<br />

this work to anaphoric reference. Priming with atypically bias-<br />

ing sentences led to interference in color naming for atypical<br />

words, whereas priming with typically biasing sentences led to<br />

facilitati<strong>on</strong> for typical words, <strong>the</strong> latter reminiscent <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Dosher<br />

and Corbett's (1982) finding. Clearly, word meaning matters;<br />

even n<strong>on</strong>color words can produce (or increase) interference--or<br />

even facilitati<strong>on</strong>--under suitable priming c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Also important is <strong>the</strong> subject's strategy. Henik et al. (1983)<br />

dem<strong>on</strong>strated that when subjects named primes before <strong>the</strong><br />

color-naming trial, normal interference was observed. How-<br />

ever, when subjects examined <strong>the</strong> primes for specified target<br />

letters, very little interference was observed. A corresp<strong>on</strong>ding<br />

pattern occurred for facilitati<strong>on</strong> in lexical decisi<strong>on</strong>. Again<br />

meaning matters: Diverting <strong>the</strong> subject from thinking <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong><br />

meaning <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> prime word virtually eliminated <strong>the</strong> usual ef-<br />

fects.<br />

Much <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> research <strong>on</strong> word meaning has been in <strong>the</strong> pie-<br />

ture-word task, where <strong>the</strong> meaning <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> both <strong>the</strong> picture and <strong>the</strong><br />

word---and <strong>the</strong>ir relati<strong>on</strong>--can be varied. Probably <strong>the</strong> best<br />

known work using this task was by Lupker (1979; Lupker &<br />

Katz, 1981, 1982). In his 1979 paper, he c<strong>on</strong>firmed Rosinski's<br />

(1977) finding that same-category words magnified interfer-<br />

ence relative to ei<strong>the</strong>r unrelated words or pr<strong>on</strong>ounceable n<strong>on</strong>-<br />

words, which caused equivalent interference. Categorical rela-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong>s have a similar effect in <strong>the</strong> "flanker" task where both<br />

stimuli are words (Shaffer & LaBerge, 1979).<br />

Lupker went <strong>on</strong> to show that associati<strong>on</strong> between <strong>the</strong> picture<br />

and <strong>the</strong> word did not increase interference, nor did instance<br />

typicality modulate <strong>the</strong> extra interference as a result <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> shared<br />

category. Thus, some semantic variables are not potent. Word<br />

imageability was a critical determinant in Lupker's study, which<br />

led him to suggest that <strong>the</strong> word's relevance to resp<strong>on</strong>se de-<br />

mands was crucial, as in <strong>the</strong> color-word task. Lupker (1979, p.<br />

494) went so far as to maintain that <strong>the</strong> two tasks were "nothing<br />

more than two sides <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> same coin:'<br />

Smith and Magee (1980; see also Magee, 1982) dem<strong>on</strong>strated<br />

that although inc<strong>on</strong>gruent words interfere with picture naming,<br />

inc<strong>on</strong>gruent pictures interfere with word categorizati<strong>on</strong>. They<br />

argued that pictures gain access to semantic informati<strong>on</strong> more<br />

readily than to name informati<strong>on</strong>, but <strong>the</strong> reverse is true for<br />

words. Glaser and Glaser 0989) took this argument c<strong>on</strong>sider-<br />

ably far<strong>the</strong>r, building a model for naming versus categorizing.<br />

These studies, like that <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Henik et al. 0983) with <strong>the</strong> color-<br />

word task, highlight <strong>the</strong> point that <strong>the</strong> task required <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> sub-<br />

ject when faced with a multidimensi<strong>on</strong>al stimulus is a crucial<br />

determinant <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> pattern <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interference observed.<br />

The empirical generalizati<strong>on</strong> is straightforward: A color-<br />

unrelated word can be made to cause greater interference (or facili-<br />

tati<strong>on</strong>, or both) with color naming if its meaning is activated by a<br />

related word or phrase shortly before <strong>the</strong> color-naming trial. Re-<br />

lated words and primed words are hard to ignore. Certainly,<br />

questi<strong>on</strong>s remain to be addressed here (e.g., <strong>the</strong> time course <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

such priming effects), but <strong>the</strong> basic result is well established.<br />

C<strong>on</strong>gruency <strong>Effect</strong>s<br />

So far this review has focused <strong>on</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> interference. How-<br />

ever, <strong>the</strong>re is ano<strong>the</strong>r side to <strong>the</strong> coin: <strong>Stroop</strong> facilitati<strong>on</strong>. If <strong>the</strong><br />

wr<strong>on</strong>g word can slow ink-naming performance, <strong>the</strong>n intuitively<br />

<strong>the</strong> right word ought to speed performance. Curiously, <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>-<br />

gruent case (e.g., red in red ink, resp<strong>on</strong>d "red") was not investi-<br />

gated until <strong>the</strong> advent <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> individual-trial versi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong><br />

<strong>Stroop</strong> task. Although Langer and Rosenberg (1966) had a c<strong>on</strong>-<br />

gruent c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> sorts in <strong>the</strong>ir study, <strong>the</strong> first actual use <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

color-word c<strong>on</strong>gruent trials was by Dalrymple-Alford and Bu-<br />

dayr (1966). Their inclusi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>gruent items am<strong>on</strong>g inc<strong>on</strong>-<br />

gruent <strong>on</strong>es <strong>on</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> cards made little difference in overall<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se time.<br />

Shortly <strong>the</strong>reafter, Sichel and Chandler (1969) used <strong>the</strong> indi-<br />

vidual-item procedure, and reported that color naming was in<br />

fact faster for c<strong>on</strong>gruent items (645 ms) than for inc<strong>on</strong>gruent<br />

items (777 ms), but that both showed interference relative to a


XXXX-type c<strong>on</strong>trol (541 ms). Thus, it is more reas<strong>on</strong>able to say<br />

that <strong>the</strong>re was reduced interference ra<strong>the</strong>r than true facilitati<strong>on</strong><br />

for <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>gruent c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. Perhaps <strong>the</strong> extent <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> apparent<br />

facilitati<strong>on</strong> depends <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> choice <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>trol c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>; recall<br />

that Dalrymple-Alford (1972a) observed no facilitati<strong>on</strong> relative<br />

to a XXXX c<strong>on</strong>trol but reliable facilitati<strong>on</strong> relative to an unre-<br />

lated-word c<strong>on</strong>trol.<br />

Although <strong>the</strong>re were reports <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interference in <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>gruent<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> (e.g., Nealis, 1973; Schulz, 1979), more <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>ten some<br />

degree <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> facilitati<strong>on</strong> was reported (e.g., Kalk<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>en, 1969). One<br />

complicati<strong>on</strong> was whe<strong>the</strong>r c<strong>on</strong>gruent and inc<strong>on</strong>gruent items<br />

were mixed or separated (blocked), as indicated in <strong>the</strong> exchange<br />

between Smith (1970) and Kalk<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>en (1970). Indeed, using a<br />

spatial analog, Logan and Zbrod<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>f(1979) showed that <strong>the</strong> pro-<br />

porti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>gruent trials over <strong>the</strong> entire task affects process-<br />

ing time (see Probability <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Various Trial Types). A fur<strong>the</strong>r com-<br />

plicati<strong>on</strong> is that c<strong>on</strong>gruence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> word and color across successive<br />

trials can also have an impact (Ettler & Rabenstein, 1979).<br />

Dyer (1973a) presented word and color <strong>on</strong> different sides <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

<strong>the</strong> fixati<strong>on</strong> point and observed 26 ms <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> facilitati<strong>on</strong> for <strong>the</strong><br />

c<strong>on</strong>gruent case (compared with a XXXX c<strong>on</strong>trol). Interference<br />

was 48 ms in <strong>the</strong> inc<strong>on</strong>gruent case. In a same-different analog<br />

also using <strong>the</strong> XXXX c<strong>on</strong>trol, Dyer (1973b) found significant<br />

interference (179 ms) and facilitati<strong>on</strong> (55 ms). Recalling<br />

Dalrymple-Alford's (1972a) findings <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 131 ms <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interference<br />

and 63 ms <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> facilitati<strong>on</strong>, it is clear that <strong>the</strong>re is usually much<br />

less facilitati<strong>on</strong> than interference (see also Dyer, 1974).<br />

Hintzman et al. (1972) reported 42 ms <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> facilitati<strong>on</strong> relative<br />

to a scrambled color-word c<strong>on</strong>trol or a "neutral" c<strong>on</strong>trol, both<br />

retaining <strong>the</strong> first letters <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> color words. My c<strong>on</strong>cerns are that (a)<br />

such c<strong>on</strong>trols would <strong>the</strong>mselves be expected to interfere, so <strong>the</strong><br />

facilitati<strong>on</strong> might disappear with a more standard c<strong>on</strong>trol, and<br />

(b) <strong>the</strong> claim that neutral words do not interfere in <strong>the</strong> individ-<br />

ual-trial versi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> task (c<strong>on</strong>trary to Klein's finding that<br />

<strong>the</strong>y do in <strong>the</strong> list versi<strong>on</strong>) is suspect given <strong>the</strong>ir choice <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>-<br />

trol c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. Streng<strong>the</strong>ning my suspici<strong>on</strong>s, Redding and Ger-<br />

jets (1977) found facilitati<strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>gruent c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> relative<br />

to scrambled color words and neutral words, but not relative to<br />

n<strong>on</strong>word c<strong>on</strong>trols.<br />

In her dissertati<strong>on</strong>, Regan (1979) obtained facilitati<strong>on</strong> for<br />

c<strong>on</strong>gruent words and interference for neutral words; she also<br />

obtained facilitati<strong>on</strong> using just <strong>the</strong> first letter <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> correct<br />

color name (see also Regan, 1978). Yet turning <strong>the</strong> words upside<br />

down and backward to slow reading, Dunbar and MacLeod<br />

(1984) obtained little facilitati<strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>gruent c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong><br />

despite huge interference in <strong>the</strong> inc<strong>on</strong>gruent c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. In fact,<br />

<strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>gruent c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> was almost identical to <strong>the</strong> XXXX<br />

c<strong>on</strong>trol c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> across our experiments.<br />

Glaser and Glaser (1982) obtained 16 ms <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> facilitati<strong>on</strong> and<br />

72 ms <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interference, relative to a XXXX-type c<strong>on</strong>trol, when<br />

<strong>the</strong> color and word were simultaneous and <strong>the</strong> task was color<br />

naming. Thomas's (1977) dissertati<strong>on</strong> presents similar data.<br />

This asymmetry is by now <strong>the</strong> expected pattern: Facilitati<strong>on</strong> is<br />

not a necessary c<strong>on</strong>comitant <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interference, although this may<br />

have more to do with measurement than with a true <strong>the</strong>oretical<br />

decoupling.<br />

With a neutral word as <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>trol c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, Duncan-John-<br />

s<strong>on</strong> and Kopell (1980, 1981) found reliable facilitati<strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong><br />

c<strong>on</strong>gruent c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> (and str<strong>on</strong>g interference in <strong>the</strong> inc<strong>on</strong>-<br />

THE STROOP EFFECT 175<br />

gruent c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>). However, <strong>the</strong>y observed no change in P300<br />

latency, an event-related brain potential assumed to be a "pure"<br />

measure <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> stimulus processing. On this basis, <strong>the</strong>y favored a<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se-competiti<strong>on</strong> account <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> effect, as did<br />

Warren and Marsh (1979) in a similar study.<br />

Rayner and Posnansky (1978) obtained facilitati<strong>on</strong> for pseu-<br />

dohomoph<strong>on</strong>es (e.g., bloo) in <strong>the</strong> picture-word task, but <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

use <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> blocked trial types allowed a strategy <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> resp<strong>on</strong>ding by<br />

reading <strong>the</strong> words. Mixing trial types, Dennis and Newstead<br />

(1981) obtained facilitati<strong>on</strong> for color words but not pseudoho-<br />

moph<strong>on</strong>es, although both produced interference. This suggests<br />

that <strong>the</strong> slow-to-read pseudohomoph<strong>on</strong>es could not be pro-<br />

cessed sufficiently quickly to assist color naming in accord with<br />

a relative speed-<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>-processing account.<br />

There have also been dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> facilitati<strong>on</strong> in <strong>Stroop</strong><br />

analogs. Certainly, this is evident in <strong>the</strong> picture-word task (e.g,<br />

Ehri, 1976; Golink<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>f& Rosinski, 1976; Underwood, 1976). For<br />

example, Ehri (1976) reported facilitati<strong>on</strong> in picture naming for<br />

c<strong>on</strong>gruent pictures and words using a picture-<strong>on</strong>ly c<strong>on</strong>trol.<br />

More recently, Glaser and Diingelh<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>f (1984) showed reliable<br />

facilitati<strong>on</strong> for <strong>the</strong> correct name <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a picture relative to a<br />

XXXX c<strong>on</strong>trol, although <strong>the</strong> facilitati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 44 ms was substan-<br />

tially less than <strong>the</strong> interference <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 131 ms caused by a different<br />

member <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> same category.<br />

Using color naming <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> seas<strong>on</strong>-appropriate or inappropriate<br />

words, Seymour (1977) obtained facilitati<strong>on</strong> plus interference.<br />

Using <strong>the</strong> arrows task, Clark and Brownell (1975) found sub-<br />

jects to be faster to identify which way an arrow was pointing<br />

(up or down) if it was located in <strong>the</strong> appropriate place in space<br />

(top or bottom, respectively). For letter identificati<strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong> pres-<br />

ence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> irrelevant letters, Taylor (1977) observed reliable facilita-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong> as well. Of course, facilitati<strong>on</strong> is very well known in <strong>the</strong><br />

word-priming domain.<br />

To summarize, here is <strong>the</strong> general c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong>: C<strong>on</strong>gruence<br />

between <strong>the</strong> irrelevant word and <strong>the</strong> to-be-named ink color <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>ten<br />

produces facilitati<strong>on</strong>. However, this facilitati<strong>on</strong> is much less than<br />

<strong>the</strong> corresp<strong>on</strong>ding interference in <strong>the</strong> inc<strong>on</strong>gruent c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, and<br />

<strong>the</strong> choice <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>trol c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> may be crucial Partly, this is a<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sequence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> relatively smaller amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> facilitati<strong>on</strong><br />

usually obtained, itself no doubt resulting partly from <strong>the</strong> prob-<br />

lem <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> trying to speed up an already rapid naming resp<strong>on</strong>se.<br />

It would be interesting to perform a color-word study al<strong>on</strong>g<br />

<strong>the</strong> lines <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> recent work by La Heij and Vermeij (1987),<br />

which showed that, whe<strong>the</strong>r reading words or naming pictures,<br />

interference decreases and facilitati<strong>on</strong> increases as a functi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

increasing target set size. The small facilitati<strong>on</strong> we have come to<br />

expect may be at least partly a c<strong>on</strong>sequence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> small set size<br />

usually used in <strong>Stroop</strong> experiments. 7<br />

Integrati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> Two Dimensi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

So far in this secti<strong>on</strong>, I have c<strong>on</strong>sidered studies focusing <strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> relevant dimensi<strong>on</strong> (color) and <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> irrelevant dimensi<strong>on</strong><br />

(word). The <strong>on</strong>ly aspect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> single trial that remains to be<br />

7 Note, however, that Marina Vanayan and I have data from <strong>the</strong> pic-<br />

ture-word task showing an increase in interference with increasing set<br />

size, so this matter is not resolved as yet.


176 COLIN M. MACLEOD<br />

dealt with is <strong>the</strong> relati<strong>on</strong> between <strong>the</strong> two dimensi<strong>on</strong>s. On a<br />

given trial, <strong>the</strong> two dimensi<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> stimulus can be more or<br />

less perceptually integrated. We know that stimulus integrati<strong>on</strong><br />

can be a powerful determiner <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> performance in many tasks<br />

(see Garner, 1974); how much does it influence <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> ef-<br />

fect?<br />

Despite relatively little published work, this issue must be<br />

addressed if we are to compare various versi<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong><br />

task meaningfully. For instance, <strong>the</strong> word is not integrated into<br />

<strong>the</strong> picture in <strong>the</strong> picture-word task to <strong>the</strong> same extent that it is<br />

integrated into <strong>the</strong> color in <strong>the</strong> standard color-word task. Fur-<br />

<strong>the</strong>rmore, if color/picture and word are separated, as in SOA<br />

studies (e$., Glaser & Glaser, 1982; Glaser & Dfingelh<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>f, 1984)<br />

or in <strong>the</strong> bar-above-word procedure (Kahneman & Chajczyk,<br />

1983), do results from <strong>the</strong>se modificati<strong>on</strong>s translate readily<br />

back to <strong>the</strong> original task?<br />

There were some hints as to <strong>the</strong> importance <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> integrati<strong>on</strong><br />

even 30 years ago (e.g., Gardner et al., 1959; Gardner & L<strong>on</strong>g,<br />

1962). More recently, though, Flowers and Stoup (1977) ob-<br />

served that interference persisted virtually unchanged over four<br />

practice sessi<strong>on</strong>s in a card-sorting task where word and color<br />

were integrated (i.e., <strong>the</strong> word red printed in green ink), but<br />

vanished with <strong>the</strong> same amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> practice when <strong>the</strong>y were not<br />

integrated (i.e., <strong>the</strong> word red inside a green-outlined rectangle).<br />

Moreover, interference was greater in <strong>the</strong> integrated case.<br />

Although <strong>the</strong>re is no doubt that n<strong>on</strong>integrated <strong>Stroop</strong> stimuli<br />

can show interference and facilitati<strong>on</strong> (Dyer, 1973a; Gatti &<br />

Egeth, 1978; Glaser & Glaser, 1982; Kahneman & Chajczyk,<br />

1983; Merikle & Gorewich, 1979), this does not mean that (all<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>) <strong>the</strong> same mechanisms are involved. As Kahneman and<br />

Henik (1981) dem<strong>on</strong>strated, if <strong>the</strong> incompatible word and color<br />

patch appear in <strong>the</strong> same attended locati<strong>on</strong>, <strong>the</strong>re is more inter-<br />

ference (202 ms) than if <strong>the</strong>y appear in adjacent locati<strong>on</strong>s, <strong>on</strong>ly<br />

<strong>on</strong>e <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> which is <strong>the</strong> focus <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> attenti<strong>on</strong> (50 ms). Kahneman and<br />

Henik also observed that compatible words facilitated color<br />

naming if <strong>the</strong> words were in <strong>the</strong> attended locati<strong>on</strong> (48 ms), but<br />

interfered if <strong>the</strong>y were n<strong>on</strong>attended (38 ms).<br />

Bradlyn and Rollins (1980) obtained interference in <strong>the</strong> inte-<br />

grated versi<strong>on</strong> but not in a n<strong>on</strong>integrated <strong>on</strong>e (<strong>the</strong> word in black<br />

ink above a color patch); however, this result appears not to<br />

generalize. Thus, Gatti and Egeth (1978) observed a decrease in<br />

interference from 90 to 40 ms as <strong>the</strong> word moved far<strong>the</strong>r from<br />

<strong>the</strong> to-be-named color patch (from <strong>on</strong>e to five degrees), but all<br />

cases produced reliable interference. The integrated versi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

<strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> task,is harder, but both versi<strong>on</strong>s produce interfer-<br />

ence.<br />

Although it seems that n<strong>on</strong>integrated stimuli do produce in-<br />

terference fairly reliably, <strong>the</strong> amount is relatively small, appar-<br />

ently falling <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>fas separati<strong>on</strong> increases. Using <strong>the</strong> picture-word<br />

task, Underwood (1976) argued that locati<strong>on</strong>al uncertainty<br />

with respect to <strong>the</strong> two dimensi<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> task was very influen-<br />

tial. When subjects knew where to expect <strong>the</strong> relevant picture<br />

versus <strong>the</strong> irrelevant word, <strong>the</strong>y showed a small amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> inter-<br />

ference from <strong>the</strong> competing word. When <strong>the</strong>y did not know<br />

where to expect picture versus word, interference was c<strong>on</strong>sider-<br />

ably greater. Apparently, not being able to prepare for where to<br />

direct attenti<strong>on</strong> makes <strong>the</strong> separated case more analogous to <strong>the</strong><br />

integrated case.<br />

What is needed is a systematic parametric comparis<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong><br />

two versi<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> task, perhaps manipulating practice as in<br />

<strong>the</strong> Flowers and Stoup (1977) study. Until such time, <strong>the</strong> extent<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> importance <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> dimensi<strong>on</strong>al integrati<strong>on</strong> is difficult to ascer-<br />

tain. Certainly, though, it seems to matter, so here is my some-<br />

what tentative general c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong>: If <strong>the</strong> to-be-named color and<br />

<strong>the</strong> to-be-ignored word are presented in separate spatial locati<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

interference will be reduced (but not eliminated) relative to <strong>the</strong><br />

standard, integrated versi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> task. Locati<strong>on</strong>al uncertainty<br />

makes an important c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong> in n<strong>on</strong>integrated situati<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

We have seen what happens when <strong>the</strong> dimensi<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a single<br />

<strong>Stroop</strong> trial are varied; let us now examine what happens with<br />

manipulati<strong>on</strong>s intended to affect <strong>the</strong> entire task.<br />

Experiment-Wide Manipulati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Informati<strong>on</strong><br />

This secti<strong>on</strong> focuses <strong>on</strong> variables affecting more than <strong>on</strong>e<br />

trial in a <strong>Stroop</strong> experiment. Some <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>se variables--such as<br />

<strong>the</strong> presence or probability <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> certain trial types--<strong>on</strong>ly exist<br />

with reference to more than <strong>on</strong>e trial. O<strong>the</strong>rs--such as SOA--<br />

occur at <strong>the</strong> level <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a single trial, but have broader implicati<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

These variables all may be thought <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> as affecting how <strong>the</strong> sub-<br />

ject approaches <strong>the</strong> task as a whole.<br />

Probability <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Various Trial Types<br />

Probably <strong>the</strong> most straightforward way to set up a <strong>Stroop</strong><br />

experiment is <strong>Stroop</strong>'s (1935b). He used four c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s, each in<br />

an independent block <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> trials: inc<strong>on</strong>gruent color naming, c<strong>on</strong>-<br />

trol color naming, inc<strong>on</strong>gruent word reading, and c<strong>on</strong>trol word<br />

reading. At first glance, this might also appear to be <strong>the</strong> "safest"<br />

way, reducing <strong>the</strong> likelihood <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> subjects adopting special strate-<br />

gies to deal with a mixture <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> trial types. Does blocking c<strong>on</strong>di-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong>s minimize strategy use or does it promote creating a spe-<br />

cial strategy for this special case? The most reassuring results<br />

are those that hold up under both mixed and blocked c<strong>on</strong>di-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong>s. No doubt, too, <strong>the</strong> compositi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> entire set <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> trials<br />

influences <strong>the</strong> subject's strategy. In <strong>the</strong> limit, if all trials in a<br />

block were c<strong>on</strong>gruent and <strong>the</strong> task was to name <strong>the</strong> ink colors,<br />

clever subjects could decrease <strong>the</strong>ir resp<strong>on</strong>se time by reading<br />

<strong>the</strong> words instead. Such a strategy would not work if all <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong><br />

trials were inc<strong>on</strong>gruent. The questi<strong>on</strong>, <strong>the</strong>n, is what happens at<br />

intermediate values.<br />

I have already menti<strong>on</strong>ed that Dalrymple-Alford and Budayr<br />

(1966) found no change in interference when some c<strong>on</strong>gruent<br />

trials were added to a card made up largely <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> inc<strong>on</strong>gruent<br />

trials, but <strong>the</strong>y did not systematically manipulate <strong>the</strong> frequency<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> trial types. Zajano and Gorman (1986) showed that total<br />

color-naming time per card decreased as more c<strong>on</strong>gruent trials<br />

were added to <strong>the</strong> card. Shor (1975) used Hamers's (1973) high-<br />

low auditory analog and found more interference when half <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

<strong>the</strong> trials were c<strong>on</strong>gruent and half inc<strong>on</strong>gruent than when all <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

<strong>the</strong> trials were inc<strong>on</strong>gruent. This counterintuitive result might<br />

be a c<strong>on</strong>sequence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> subjects listening to <strong>the</strong> word and saying its<br />

opposite in <strong>the</strong> inc<strong>on</strong>gruent case, a strategy that could be elimi-<br />

nated by using more than two resp<strong>on</strong>ses. My guess is that Shor's<br />

result would reverse itself with such a modificati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Logan and Zbrod<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>f(1979) used <strong>the</strong> above/below spatial an-<br />

alog <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> task where subjects are to read <strong>the</strong> word and<br />

ignore its spatial locati<strong>on</strong>. (Note that in this task interference


occurs in reading <strong>the</strong> word, unlike in <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> task.) As <strong>the</strong><br />

frequency <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> inc<strong>on</strong>gruent trials increased from 10% to 90% over<br />

experiments, resp<strong>on</strong>se times shifted from c<strong>on</strong>gruent trials be-<br />

ing faster to inc<strong>on</strong>gruent trials being faster. Logan and Zbro-<br />

d<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>f were careful to point out that low error rates indicated<br />

subjects were not simply "cheating" by attending to <strong>the</strong> suppos-<br />

edly unattended dimensi<strong>on</strong>. Apparently, subjects adopted a<br />

strategy <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> dividing attenti<strong>on</strong> over <strong>the</strong> two dimensi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Still it would be better to use a larger set <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> items to preclude<br />

<strong>the</strong> strategies possible with <strong>on</strong>ly two stimuli. Lowe and Mitterer<br />

(1982) used three ink colors (red, blue, and green), <strong>the</strong> three<br />

corresp<strong>on</strong>ding color words, and three neutral words (far, most,<br />

and slant). On each trial, two words were presented. The target<br />

was always a neutral word in color, and <strong>the</strong> subject was to name<br />

<strong>the</strong> ink color. The o<strong>the</strong>r word was always printed in black ink,<br />

but could be a color word or neutral word. When this irrelevant<br />

word was a color word, it could ei<strong>the</strong>r be c<strong>on</strong>gruent or inc<strong>on</strong>-<br />

gruent with <strong>the</strong> target ink color.<br />

Lowe and Mitterer manipulated <strong>the</strong> proporti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>gruent<br />

stimuli in steps <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> .25 from 0 to .75. Relative to <strong>the</strong> neutral<br />

c<strong>on</strong>trol, interference increased from 35 ms with 0% c<strong>on</strong>gruent<br />

trials to 66 ms with 75% c<strong>on</strong>gruent trials; however, facilitati<strong>on</strong><br />

hardly changed (12 to 17 ms). This was taken as evidence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

automatic word encoding in both c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s, but with a fur<strong>the</strong>r<br />

c<strong>on</strong>flict-resoluti<strong>on</strong> process in <strong>the</strong> inc<strong>on</strong>gruent case following<br />

Taylor's (1977) arguments. Note, too, <strong>the</strong> different effects <strong>on</strong><br />

facilitati<strong>on</strong> and interference.<br />

Although <strong>the</strong>re are limited data, it seems that <strong>the</strong> composi-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> entire set <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> trials does matter. As <strong>the</strong> proporti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

c<strong>on</strong>gruent trials increases, <strong>the</strong> "irrelevant" word becomes pro-<br />

gressively more relevant. Here, <strong>the</strong>n, is <strong>the</strong> general c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong>:<br />

The presence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>gruent trials am<strong>on</strong>g <strong>the</strong> inc<strong>on</strong>gruent and c<strong>on</strong>-<br />

trol trials will tend to invoke <strong>the</strong> tactic <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> splitting attenti<strong>on</strong> over <strong>the</strong><br />

two dimensi<strong>on</strong>s, <strong>the</strong>reby increasing interference <strong>on</strong> inc<strong>on</strong>gruent<br />

trials. For this reas<strong>on</strong>, it is probably best to c<strong>on</strong>duct novel exper-<br />

iments initially without a c<strong>on</strong>gruent c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, and <strong>the</strong>n to add<br />

that c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> and observe whe<strong>the</strong>r its presence changes inter-<br />

ference fundamentally. Thus far very few experiments have<br />

taken this tack. We should also determine <strong>the</strong> extent to which<br />

this attenti<strong>on</strong>-splitting strategy is under subject c<strong>on</strong>trol, per-<br />

haps through a manipulati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> instructi<strong>on</strong>s or pay<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>fs.<br />

Stimulus Set Size<br />

There is typically quite a small set <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> stimuli in <strong>the</strong> color-<br />

word task and quite a large set in <strong>the</strong> picture-word task. Proba-<br />

bly this is because creating large, differentiable sets <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> items is<br />

easier with pictures than with colors. Indeed, <strong>the</strong> bulk <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong><br />

color-word studies use from two to five stimuli. We know that<br />

increases in stimulus set size increase time to name but have<br />

little impact <strong>on</strong> time to read (cf. Fraisse, 1969; Ghols<strong>on</strong> &<br />

Hohle, 1968b). C<strong>on</strong>sequently, some investigators adopted set-<br />

size manipulati<strong>on</strong>s as <strong>on</strong>e way to attack interference.<br />

<strong>An</strong> illustrati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> how few colors are typically used in <strong>the</strong><br />

<strong>Stroop</strong> task comes from a study <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> set-size effects by Golden<br />

(1974a); he found that using three, four, or five different colors<br />

made little difference in interference. O<strong>the</strong>r research supports<br />

his c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong>. Ghols<strong>on</strong> and Hohle (1968a) found that, in a<br />

choice reacti<strong>on</strong>-time versi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> task, increasing set<br />

THE STROOP EFFECT 177<br />

size from two to four increased overall resp<strong>on</strong>se time but not<br />

interference. Essentially <strong>the</strong> same result was obtained for set<br />

sizes three to five by Ray (1974) and for set sizes two to five by<br />

McClain (1983b). Although this is <strong>the</strong> usual range, I am reluc-<br />

tam to infer from such small variati<strong>on</strong>s that set size is not a<br />

factor.<br />

Until recently, <strong>the</strong> <strong>on</strong>ly discordant <strong>Stroop</strong> study was that <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

Williams (1977), who used <strong>the</strong> largest variati<strong>on</strong>: from 2 to 8.<br />

She found that both interference and facilitati<strong>on</strong> increased as<br />

set size increased. The picture becomes even more complicated<br />

as so<strong>on</strong> as we step bey<strong>on</strong>d <strong>the</strong> color-word task. In <strong>the</strong> increas-<br />

ingly comm<strong>on</strong> two-word analog, several studies have c<strong>on</strong>sis-<br />

tently found decreases in interference coupled with increases in<br />

facilitati<strong>on</strong> as set size increased. Taylor (1977) obtained this<br />

result in moving from 2 to 4 alternatives; La Heij, Van der<br />

Heijden, and Schreuder (1985) obtained it in moving from 2 to<br />

12 alternatives. How are we to bring <strong>the</strong>se disparate findings<br />

toge<strong>the</strong>r?<br />

La Heij and Vermeij (1987) recently tried to answer this ques-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong>. Varying set size from two to eight in both a letter-reading<br />

and a picture-naming task (in separate experiments), <strong>the</strong>y<br />

found that interference decreased and facilitati<strong>on</strong> increased for<br />

both types <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> materials relative to a XXXX-type c<strong>on</strong>trol. Al-<br />

though <strong>the</strong>y claim (p. 359) that it would be difficult to replicate<br />

this experiment using <strong>the</strong> color-word task, I am not c<strong>on</strong>vinced:<br />

Their pattern is evident even between set sizes two and four.<br />

Partly because <strong>the</strong>ir findings c<strong>on</strong>flict with those just reviewed,<br />

a replicati<strong>on</strong> would be useful. Results with <strong>the</strong> color-word task<br />

that corresp<strong>on</strong>ded to those <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>La Heij and Vermeij (1987) would<br />

fur<strong>the</strong>r streng<strong>the</strong>n <strong>the</strong> argument that all <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>se superficially<br />

different interference tasks in fact tap a comm<strong>on</strong> element.<br />

A general c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong> here, given <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>flict in <strong>the</strong> data base,<br />

is risky Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, <strong>the</strong>re is <strong>the</strong> issue <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> resp<strong>on</strong>se set size yet to<br />

be discussed (see Resp<strong>on</strong>se Set Size and Compositi<strong>on</strong>). So I sim-<br />

ply note that stimulus set size may influence <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> effect.<br />

Part <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> my cauti<strong>on</strong> stems from some data that Marina Vanayan<br />

and I have collected: Increasing set size in <strong>the</strong> picture-word task<br />

from 4 to 12 increased interference in our study This disagrees<br />

with La Heij and Vermeij, and we are pursuing possible reas<strong>on</strong>s<br />

for <strong>the</strong> difference. Until such discrepancies are resolved, it<br />

would be premature to <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>fer a firm statement about stimulus<br />

set-size effects.<br />

Trial Sequence<br />

People <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>ten feel particularly t<strong>on</strong>gue tied when <strong>the</strong> to-be-ig-<br />

nored word <strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong>e trial turns out to be <strong>the</strong> to-be-named color<br />

<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> next trial (e.g., green in red ink precedes blue in green<br />

ink). Having just suppressed <strong>the</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>se "green" seems to<br />

make it harder to say it <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> next trial: There are sequential<br />

effects in <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> task. Dalrymple-Alford and Budayr (1966)<br />

first pointed out that interference in <strong>the</strong> list versi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong><br />

<strong>Stroop</strong> task was greatest if each ink color was that named by <strong>the</strong><br />

immediately preceding word.<br />

The most detailed investigati<strong>on</strong>s in this area are those <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

Neill (1977, 1978; NeiU & Westberry, 1987) and Etiler (1977a,<br />

1977b, 1978b, 1980; Effier & Rabenstein, 1979). Effier (1977a)<br />

reported a "serial interference": When <strong>the</strong> irrelevant word <strong>on</strong><br />

trial n was acoustically similar to <strong>the</strong> ink color <strong>on</strong> trial n + 1,


178 COLIN M. MACLEOD<br />

interference was enhanced. C<strong>on</strong>versely, when <strong>the</strong> ink color <strong>on</strong><br />

trial n was that <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> irrelevant word <strong>on</strong> trial n + 1, interference<br />

was reduced (Effler, 1977b). He <strong>the</strong>n c<strong>on</strong>firmed and extended<br />

<strong>the</strong>se results in a variety <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> ways (Effler, 1978b, 1980; Ettler &<br />

Rabenstein, 1979).<br />

The first <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>se findings makes sense if it is assumed that<br />

<strong>the</strong> word <strong>on</strong> trial n must be suppressed, and <strong>the</strong>n has to be given<br />

additi<strong>on</strong>al activati<strong>on</strong> to become <strong>the</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>se <strong>on</strong> trial n + 1. The<br />

sec<strong>on</strong>d result can be explained as an instance where having just<br />

made a particular resp<strong>on</strong>se <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> last trial makes it easier to<br />

discard that as a possible resp<strong>on</strong>se <strong>on</strong> this trial. Of course, such<br />

explanati<strong>on</strong>s are easier after having seen <strong>the</strong> data.<br />

Neill (1977) extended <strong>the</strong> Dalrymple-Alford and Budayr<br />

(1966) observati<strong>on</strong> to <strong>the</strong> individual-trial versi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong><br />

task, finding <strong>the</strong> same result that <strong>the</strong>y and Efller (1977a) found:<br />

greater interference if <strong>the</strong> word <strong>on</strong> trial n matched <strong>the</strong> ink color<br />

<strong>on</strong> trial n + 1. Neill labeled <strong>the</strong> phenomen<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> distractor-sup-<br />

pressi<strong>on</strong> effect. We now know that <strong>the</strong> phenomen<strong>on</strong> is fairly<br />

general (e.g., Westberry, 1984), and extends to letter naming<br />

(Allport, Tipper, & Chmiel, 1985; Tipper & Cranst<strong>on</strong>, 1985)<br />

and picture naming (Allport et al., 1985; Tipper, 1985). In <strong>the</strong><br />

case <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> pictures, even n<strong>on</strong>identical members <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> same cate-<br />

gory can produce <strong>the</strong> distractor-suppressi<strong>on</strong> effect.<br />

Neill (1978) observed that when <strong>the</strong> word <strong>on</strong> trial n + 1<br />

matched <strong>the</strong> ink color just named <strong>on</strong> trial n, <strong>the</strong>re was facilita-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong>, this time replicating Effler (1977b). He took this as evi-<br />

dence that <strong>the</strong> set <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> highly activated competing resp<strong>on</strong>ses was<br />

made smaller by <strong>the</strong> overlap, hence reducing resp<strong>on</strong>se competi-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong>, as opposed to <strong>the</strong> suggesti<strong>on</strong> I <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>fered in discussing<br />

Etfler's (1977b) study.<br />

Lowe (1979, 1985) first obtained this successive-trials effect<br />

when c<strong>on</strong>secutive items were not all inc<strong>on</strong>gruent items <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong><br />

standard type used by Neill, <strong>the</strong>reby increasing its generaliza-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong>. He <strong>the</strong>n manipulated <strong>the</strong> proporti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> trials <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> various<br />

types in subsequent experiments, showing that <strong>the</strong> distractor-<br />

suppressi<strong>on</strong> effect could be eliminated or even turned into facili-<br />

tati<strong>on</strong>. This clearly implicates subject strategies in <strong>the</strong> phenome-<br />

n<strong>on</strong>, and Lowe was unc<strong>on</strong>vinced <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> need to propose an<br />

inhibitory mechanism <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> sort favored by NeiU or Tipper. He<br />

;~uggested that subjects may have been trying to match succes-<br />

sive stimuli, <strong>the</strong>reby not treating trials as independent, but later<br />

I rescinded this argument (Lowe, 1985). Efforts to make succes-<br />

sive stimuli less matchable, by using different cases and by vary-<br />

ing list compositi<strong>on</strong> and SOA, did not noticeably alter <strong>the</strong> dis-<br />

tractor-suppressi<strong>on</strong> effect. Still resisting an inhibiti<strong>on</strong> argu-<br />

ment, Lowe maintained that <strong>the</strong> problem was in coordinating<br />

<strong>the</strong> color and word codes.<br />

More recently, Neill and Wes<strong>the</strong>rry (1987) manipulated<br />

speed-accuracy instructi<strong>on</strong>s and intertrial interval. Underaccu-<br />

racy instructi<strong>on</strong>s, subjects showed <strong>the</strong> effect, but it disappeared<br />

as emphasis <strong>on</strong> speed increased. As well, <strong>the</strong> effect persisted<br />

with up to a l-s intertrial interval, but dissipated between 1 and<br />

2 s. Neill and Westberry interpreted <strong>the</strong>se findings as c<strong>on</strong>sistent<br />

with a selective-inhibiti<strong>on</strong> account: After broad activati<strong>on</strong>, se-<br />

lective inhibiti<strong>on</strong> is used to restrict processing to just <strong>the</strong> rele-<br />

vant informati<strong>on</strong>, an idea very much in accord with <strong>the</strong> work<br />

<strong>on</strong> priming o fambiguous words discussed under Semantic Vari-<br />

ati<strong>on</strong> (e.g., Oden & Spira, 1983).<br />

The work <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> distractor-suppressi<strong>on</strong> effect suggests a gen-<br />

eralizati<strong>on</strong> bey<strong>on</strong>d <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> effect. In additi<strong>on</strong> to activati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

<strong>the</strong>re may well be an inhibitory process that helps to c<strong>on</strong>strain<br />

<strong>the</strong> informati<strong>on</strong> selected for more extensive processing. For <strong>the</strong><br />

present, this is seen as an active strategy <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> inhibiti<strong>on</strong>. Whe<strong>the</strong>r<br />

<strong>the</strong> facilitati<strong>on</strong> from successive trials has to do with a functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

shrinking <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> set <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> competing resp<strong>on</strong>ses or a suppressi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ses recently uttered is less clear. My own bias, in opposi-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong> to Neill (1978), is to invoke a suppressi<strong>on</strong> idea so that <strong>the</strong><br />

facilitati<strong>on</strong> and interference effects as a result <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> item sequence<br />

have a comm<strong>on</strong> grounding.<br />

As a footnote, it is interesting to c<strong>on</strong>trast <strong>the</strong>se results with<br />

those involving a slightly different variati<strong>on</strong>. Thomas 0977)<br />

repeated <strong>the</strong> word comp<strong>on</strong>ent over successive pairs <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> color-<br />

word stimuli, and found that word repetiti<strong>on</strong> decreased both<br />

interference and facilitati<strong>on</strong>. Eftler (1980) c<strong>on</strong>firmed <strong>the</strong> inter-<br />

ference decrease, both when <strong>the</strong> word named an ink color in <strong>the</strong><br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se set and when <strong>the</strong> word named an ink color not in <strong>the</strong><br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se set. However, <strong>the</strong>re were no repetiti<strong>on</strong> effects for n<strong>on</strong>-<br />

color words. Subsequently, Ettler (1981) tried to determine<br />

whe<strong>the</strong>r amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> repetiti<strong>on</strong> over trials was important, but his<br />

odd n<strong>on</strong>m<strong>on</strong>ot<strong>on</strong>ic pattern provides little insight.<br />

Ellis<strong>on</strong> and Lambert (1968) took a ra<strong>the</strong>r different tack. They<br />

had subjects repeat <strong>the</strong> color words c<strong>on</strong>tinuously before per-<br />

forming <strong>the</strong> interference trials, with <strong>the</strong> goal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reducing <strong>the</strong><br />

meaning <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> words and hence <strong>the</strong>ir potential for interference.<br />

Although performance <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> interference card sped up, <strong>the</strong>y<br />

could not determine whe<strong>the</strong>r this was due to "semantic satia-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong>." A number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> additi<strong>on</strong>al c<strong>on</strong>trols would have been desir-<br />

able in this work. Still <strong>the</strong>ir basic finding is in line with Eftler's<br />

(1980).<br />

These two different outcomes having to do with <strong>the</strong> relati<strong>on</strong><br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> word <strong>on</strong> trial n - 1 to <strong>the</strong> critical stimulus <strong>on</strong> trial n<br />

actually fit toge<strong>the</strong>r quite nicely, so I <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>fer <strong>the</strong> following c<strong>on</strong>clu-<br />

si<strong>on</strong>: When <strong>the</strong> irrelevant word <strong>on</strong> trial n - 1 is <strong>the</strong> name <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong><br />

target ink color <strong>on</strong> trial n, interference with color naming will be<br />

enhanced temporarily; when <strong>the</strong> ink color <strong>on</strong> trial n - 1 matches<br />

<strong>the</strong> word <strong>on</strong> trial n, <strong>the</strong>re will be some facilitati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> color naming<br />

<strong>on</strong> trial n. If <strong>the</strong> word <strong>on</strong> trial n - I is repeated <strong>on</strong> trial n, <strong>the</strong>n <strong>the</strong><br />

word is already suppressed and will cause less interference in<br />

naming a different ink color <strong>on</strong> trial n. <strong>An</strong> interesting study<br />

would be to mix <strong>the</strong>se two types <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> repetiti<strong>on</strong> effects in <strong>the</strong> same<br />

experiment, directly comparing <strong>the</strong>ir size.<br />

Pretrial Cues<br />

Very closely related to studies <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong>e trial <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

next are studies <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a cue just before a <strong>Stroop</strong> tria !.<br />

Nealis (1974) tackled this problem by presenting color words in<br />

colored ink before a to-be-read word. He was able to produce a<br />

reverse <strong>Stroop</strong> effect in that <strong>the</strong>re was interference evident in<br />

reading <strong>the</strong> target word. Hint<strong>on</strong> (1976) found that prior presen-<br />

tati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> word whose ink color was to be named <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

upcoming target trial tended to decrease color-naming time<br />

(but see Trial Sequence for c<strong>on</strong>flicting results). However, when<br />

that word had to be rehearsed for later recall, resp<strong>on</strong>se time to<br />

its ink color increased. Hint<strong>on</strong> took <strong>the</strong> facilitati<strong>on</strong> effect as<br />

evidence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reduced competiti<strong>on</strong> for entry into a resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

buffer; <strong>the</strong> increased interference effect was held to be <strong>the</strong> result<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> increased problems in resp<strong>on</strong>se selecti<strong>on</strong>.


Neill (1978) used <strong>the</strong> precue as a sort <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>"warning; <strong>the</strong> most<br />

prevalent use <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> this procedure. The target was a standard in-<br />

c<strong>on</strong>gruent color-word combinati<strong>on</strong>. Neill observed roughly<br />

equivalent resp<strong>on</strong>se times after a neutral cue or <strong>the</strong> relevant<br />

color word. However, interference was increased by cueing ei-<br />

<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> wr<strong>on</strong>g ink-color name or <strong>the</strong> irrelevant color word.<br />

According to Neill, <strong>the</strong> number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> competing resp<strong>on</strong>ses and<br />

<strong>the</strong>ir strength determined interference. The results were inc<strong>on</strong>-<br />

sistent with <strong>the</strong> idea that increasing a resp<strong>on</strong>ses availability<br />

could make it easier to reject.<br />

Using <strong>the</strong> hi-lo analog, Harvey (1984) varied <strong>the</strong> interval by<br />

which a warning preceded <strong>the</strong> critical stimulus. The warning<br />

stimulus signaled <strong>the</strong> subject whe<strong>the</strong>r to resp<strong>on</strong>d to <strong>the</strong> word's<br />

meaning or its positi<strong>on</strong>. Oddly, <strong>on</strong>ly word reading showed inter-<br />

ference, which declined as <strong>the</strong> time between warning and criti-<br />

cal stimulus increased. Harvey took this as evidence that when<br />

subjects could better focus <strong>the</strong>ir attenti<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> appropriate<br />

dimensi<strong>on</strong> because <strong>the</strong>y were warned, <strong>the</strong>y were better able to<br />

cope with incompatible dimensi<strong>on</strong>s. This idea seems worthy <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

fur<strong>the</strong>r study.<br />

Logan and Zbrod<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>f (1982), in <strong>the</strong>ir spatial analog <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong><br />

<strong>Stroop</strong> task, also used <strong>the</strong> precue technique. When <strong>the</strong> cue pro-<br />

vided advance informati<strong>on</strong> about whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> upcoming trial<br />

was compatible or incompatible, resp<strong>on</strong>se time <strong>on</strong> cued trials<br />

was faster than <strong>on</strong> uncued trials. When <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e cue could<br />

appear, its benefit was greater than when two cues could ap-<br />

pear. Practice with cues permitted subjects to benefit from<br />

<strong>the</strong>m even with shorter warnings, but did not increase <strong>the</strong> bene-<br />

fit. Overall, <strong>the</strong>se results were taken to reflect successful sub-<br />

ject-initiated c<strong>on</strong>structi<strong>on</strong> and use <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> strategies. Because <strong>the</strong><br />

<strong>Stroop</strong> task is <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>ten seen as a hallmark index <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> automaticity,<br />

such dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> role <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> strategic c<strong>on</strong>trol are impor-<br />

tant.<br />

Logan, Zbrod<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>f, and Williams<strong>on</strong> (1984) pursued this using<br />

<strong>the</strong> color-word task. By making <strong>the</strong> ink color predictable from<br />

<strong>the</strong> word and <strong>the</strong>n providing advance knowledge <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> word,<br />

<strong>the</strong>y found that subjects could perform <strong>the</strong> color-naming task<br />

more rapidly with two ink colors but not with four. Their result<br />

with <strong>on</strong>ly two resp<strong>on</strong>ses is reminiscent <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Shor's (1975); <strong>the</strong> fact<br />

that <strong>the</strong> advantage <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> cue dissipated with four resp<strong>on</strong>ses<br />

suggests that <strong>the</strong> strategy could not handle <strong>the</strong> greater uncer-<br />

tainty with more stimuli.<br />

Probably <strong>the</strong> most provocative recent results are those <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

Marcel (1983, Experiment 3) <strong>on</strong> unc<strong>on</strong>scious perceptual pro-<br />

cessing. He first determined <strong>the</strong> interval required between a<br />

stimulus and a pattern mask such that a subject could not dis-<br />

criminate a word from a blank. Then he ran blocks <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> trials in<br />

which a cue word was ei<strong>the</strong>r masked with this interval, to pre-<br />

vent c<strong>on</strong>scious percepti<strong>on</strong>, or masked with a very l<strong>on</strong>g interval,<br />

so that it would be easily seen. He included c<strong>on</strong>gruent, inc<strong>on</strong>-<br />

gruent, neutral (e.g., <strong>the</strong> word water), and no-word c<strong>on</strong>trol trials,<br />

and also varied over blocks whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> word was presented<br />

simultaneously with <strong>the</strong> color patch or 400 ms earlier. Marcel<br />

found that performance in <strong>the</strong> no-word c<strong>on</strong>trol and <strong>the</strong> neutral<br />

c<strong>on</strong>trol was essentially identical. Although facilitati<strong>on</strong> and inter-<br />

ference effects were a little larger if <strong>the</strong> subject was aware--par-<br />

ticularly when <strong>the</strong> word preceded <strong>the</strong> color by 400 ms--both<br />

were present even when <strong>the</strong> subject could not report whe<strong>the</strong>r a<br />

THE STROOP EFFECT 179<br />

word cue had been present. Marcel argued against <strong>the</strong> view that<br />

interference arose in resp<strong>on</strong>se producti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

As Marcel pointed out, his was not <strong>the</strong> first study <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>"sublimi-<br />

nal percepti<strong>on</strong>" in <strong>the</strong> SWoop task. Severance and Dyer (1973)<br />

tried but did not obtain a subliminal <strong>Stroop</strong> effect. Although<br />

similar to Marcel's in some ways, <strong>the</strong>ir study used extremely<br />

brief word exposures followed by a bright blank field to obtain<br />

chance performance in pretesting. Marcel argued that <strong>the</strong>ir en-<br />

ergy masking has its effect much more peripherally than does<br />

his pattern masking, so <strong>the</strong> difference in results is not that sur-<br />

prising.<br />

Cheesman and Merikle (1984) examined Marcers subliminal<br />

<strong>Stroop</strong> effect using a more stringent forced-choice method for<br />

obtaining <strong>the</strong> optimal threshold <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> awareness <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> cue word.<br />

Then, using four colors, <strong>the</strong>y varied <strong>the</strong> cue-word detectability<br />

from 25% (chance) to 100% (unmasked prime). At 25%, <strong>the</strong>re<br />

was no effect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> cue word; as cue-word detectability in-<br />

creased, so did facilitati<strong>on</strong> and interference. They claimed that<br />

<strong>the</strong>ir findings refuted Marcel's claim <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> unc<strong>on</strong>scious percepti<strong>on</strong><br />

in <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> task.<br />

Cheesman and Merikle (1984) went <strong>on</strong> to dem<strong>on</strong>strate that<br />

<strong>the</strong>y could obtain effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> cue at several SOAs when <strong>the</strong>y<br />

used a subjective threshold in which subjects claimed not to see<br />

<strong>the</strong> cue word. Subsequently, Cheesman and Merikle (1986) dem-<br />

<strong>on</strong>strated that subjective thresholds did c<strong>on</strong>vey some informa-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong> about processing. First, <strong>the</strong>y showed that <strong>the</strong> cue word had<br />

an effect whe<strong>the</strong>r it was above or below subjective threshold (so<br />

l<strong>on</strong>g as it was above objective threshold). They <strong>the</strong>n varied <strong>the</strong><br />

proporti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> trials in which <strong>the</strong> cue and target were c<strong>on</strong>gruent,<br />

and discovered that subjects could use this variati<strong>on</strong> to assist<br />

<strong>the</strong>ir processing, but <strong>on</strong>ly if <strong>the</strong> cue word was above subjective<br />

threshold. In o<strong>the</strong>r words, a strategy could be implemented <strong>on</strong>ly<br />

when subjects were aware <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> primes, extending <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>clu-<br />

si<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Logan and Zbrod<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>f (1982).<br />

Although <strong>the</strong>re is not a massive amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> evidence here, I<br />

will dignify <strong>the</strong> clear picture with a c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong>: Advance cues<br />

c<strong>on</strong>veying informati<strong>on</strong> about <strong>the</strong> upcoming <strong>Stroop</strong> trial can be<br />

used to establish processing strategies that improve performance<br />

if <strong>the</strong>se cues are above <strong>the</strong> level <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> subjective awareness and if a<br />

very small set <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> cues is used c<strong>on</strong>sistently. The major implicati<strong>on</strong><br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> work in this secti<strong>on</strong> is that foreknowledge can permit<br />

subjects to c<strong>on</strong>struct a beneficial, task-wide strategy. Atten-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong>al-allocati<strong>on</strong> policy is a critical element in <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> task.<br />

Stimulus Onset Asynchr<strong>on</strong>y (SOA)<br />

Many explanati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>Stroop</strong> interference have relied <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

relative speed <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> two competing stimulus di-<br />

mensi<strong>on</strong>s. Intuitively, ifa subject processes two dimensi<strong>on</strong>s si-<br />

multaneously, <strong>on</strong>e could interfere with <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r, especially if<br />

<strong>the</strong> dimensi<strong>on</strong> that is processed faster is <strong>the</strong> <strong>on</strong>e that is sup-<br />

posed to be ignored (see Dunbar & MacLeod, 1984, for a dis-<br />

cussi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> "horse-race" metaphor). Logically, <strong>the</strong>n, giving<br />

<strong>the</strong> slower color dimensi<strong>on</strong> a head start should reduce <strong>the</strong> im-<br />

pact <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> ordinarily faster word processing, <strong>the</strong>reby decreas-<br />

ing interference (and facilitati<strong>on</strong>). With <strong>the</strong> individual-trial pro-<br />

cedure, such studies can be performed by manipulating <strong>the</strong><br />

SOA between <strong>the</strong> word and color.<br />

The first study to vary SOA was by Dyer (197 lc). He used a


180 COLIN M. MACLEOD<br />

large SOA range (0-500 ms) because he thought it possible that<br />

short SOAs would lead to increased interference but l<strong>on</strong>ger<br />

SOAs would lead to decreased interference. With four comm<strong>on</strong><br />

colors and an XXXX-type c<strong>on</strong>trol, he explored both inc<strong>on</strong>-<br />

gruent and c<strong>on</strong>gruent combinati<strong>on</strong>s by having a word appear in<br />

black at 1 <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 10 intervals before that same word appeared in<br />

color. The subject's task was to name <strong>the</strong> ink color.<br />

Dyer (197 l c) found facilitati<strong>on</strong> for c<strong>on</strong>gruent trials (26 ms)<br />

and interference for inc<strong>on</strong>gruent trials (75 ms), relative to <strong>the</strong><br />

c<strong>on</strong>trol, at all word-preexposure intervals. However, <strong>the</strong> time by<br />

which <strong>the</strong> word preceded <strong>the</strong> ink color also mattered. Interfer-<br />

ence was about 90 ms with a 0-ms SOA, increased to I 18 ms at a<br />

40-ms SOA, fell to 68 ms at a 200-ms SOA, and <strong>the</strong>n fur<strong>the</strong>r<br />

decreased to 32 ms at a 500-ms SOA. Although a n<strong>on</strong>m<strong>on</strong>oto-<br />

nicity is suggested, in fact Dyer's data <strong>on</strong>ly allowed <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>clu-<br />

si<strong>on</strong> that interference decreased with SOA. Never<strong>the</strong>less, Dyer<br />

claimed that interference peaked in <strong>the</strong> neighborhood <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a 50-<br />

ms preexposure <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> word. He also noted that facilitati<strong>on</strong><br />

increased somewhat from <strong>the</strong> 0-ms to <strong>the</strong> 200-ms SOA.<br />

Ra<strong>the</strong>r than preexposing a color word in black and <strong>the</strong>n<br />

showing that color word in an ink color, Dyer and Severance<br />

(1973) preexposed a color word in Mack and <strong>the</strong>n showed a<br />

XXXX-type stimulus in an ink color. Thus, <strong>the</strong> color and word<br />

were now not integrated. Relative to <strong>the</strong> earlier study, this cut<br />

interference almost in half and eliminated facilitati<strong>on</strong>. Fur<strong>the</strong>r-<br />

more, varying SOA from 0 to 100 ms had no effect <strong>on</strong> interfer-<br />

ence. When Severance and Dyer (1973) tried to replicate <strong>the</strong>se<br />

findings with a "subliminal" preexposure <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> irrelevant<br />

word, <strong>the</strong>y found no interference.<br />

In 1974, Dyer tried using much l<strong>on</strong>ger SOAs between <strong>the</strong><br />

word and <strong>the</strong> ink color. Interference decreased to a minimal<br />

level with a 2-s SOA, but <strong>the</strong>n increased again as SOA grew<br />

bey<strong>on</strong>d 2 s. Facilitati<strong>on</strong> looked to follow <strong>the</strong> same pattern but<br />

with diminished magnitude. Relative to randomized SOAs,<br />

blocking SOA produced faster color naming at short SOAs, but<br />

<strong>the</strong> effect <strong>on</strong> interference was minimal. Taken toge<strong>the</strong>r, all <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

Dyer's SOA results suggest an el<strong>on</strong>gated S-shaped time course<br />

in <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> effect: Interference initially increased with greater<br />

SOA, <strong>the</strong>n decreased, and finally increased again at very l<strong>on</strong>g<br />

SOAs. Such a pattern is not easily rec<strong>on</strong>ciled with any simple<br />

relative speed-<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>-processing interpretati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

In a <strong>the</strong>sis modeled after Dyer's work, Thomas (1977) varied<br />

SOA from <strong>the</strong> ink color leading <strong>the</strong> word by 300 ms to <strong>the</strong> word<br />

leading <strong>the</strong> ink color by 300 ms. Interference with color naming<br />

was greater for shorter SOAs. This result was also extended to<br />

facilitati<strong>on</strong> and c<strong>on</strong>firmed by Neumann (1980), Goolkasian<br />

(1981), and L<strong>on</strong>g and Lyman (1987). Precisely <strong>the</strong> same pattern<br />

was reported by Flowers (1975) in a sorting versi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> color-<br />

word task; Flowers and Wilcox (1982) extended this to <strong>the</strong><br />

flanker procedure using two letters or digits. In Thomas's study,<br />

facilitati<strong>on</strong> occurred whenever <strong>the</strong> word preceded or even when<br />

it closely followed <strong>the</strong> corresp<strong>on</strong>ding ink color (up to 50 ms<br />

after). Overall, <strong>the</strong> evidence was not c<strong>on</strong>sistent with a simple<br />

differential rate-<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>-processing model.<br />

Lassen (1975) extended SOA studies to <strong>the</strong> picture-word<br />

task. Interference with picture naming peaked when <strong>the</strong> inc<strong>on</strong>-<br />

gruent word led <strong>the</strong> picture by about 100 ms. On <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r<br />

hand, although pictures did cause a small amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interfer-<br />

ence with word reading, manipulati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> SOA had no effect <strong>on</strong><br />

reading words. These results are reminiscent <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> data found<br />

in <strong>the</strong> standard <strong>Stroop</strong> task, as are Magee's (1982) data <strong>on</strong> SOA<br />

manipulati<strong>on</strong>s in <strong>the</strong> picture-word task.<br />

The definitive pair <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> studies <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> relati<strong>on</strong> between SOA<br />

and interference has been reported quite recently Glaser and<br />

Glaser (1982, Experiment 1) used <strong>the</strong> color-word task and in-<br />

cluded inc<strong>on</strong>gruent, c<strong>on</strong>trol, and c<strong>on</strong>gruent trials. When <strong>the</strong><br />

task was to read <strong>the</strong> word, <strong>the</strong>re was no interference or facilita-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong> regardless <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> whe<strong>the</strong>r ink color or word was presented up to<br />

400 ms earlier: SOA had no effect <strong>on</strong> word reading. However,<br />

SOA had a powerful effect <strong>on</strong> ink-color naming. If <strong>the</strong> word was<br />

presented before or even as late as 100 ms after <strong>the</strong> ink color,<br />

both interference and facilitati<strong>on</strong> occurred. Interference was<br />

maximal in a 100-ms window around simultaneous, c<strong>on</strong>sistent<br />

with Dyer's results.<br />

In o<strong>the</strong>r experiments, Glaser and Glaser (1982) showed that<br />

increasing <strong>the</strong> frequency <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>gruent trials could lead to a<br />

reverse <strong>Stroop</strong> effect (ink color interferes with word reading),<br />

with both interference and facilitati<strong>on</strong> in word reading when<br />

<strong>the</strong> word preceded <strong>the</strong> ink color by 200 ms or more. They ar-<br />

gued that <strong>the</strong> high probability <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> "irrelevant" word also<br />

being <strong>the</strong> name <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> ink color led subjects to prepare <strong>the</strong> word<br />

for output. However, <strong>the</strong>ir main c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong> was that <strong>the</strong> SOA<br />

pattern did not readily fit a simple speed-<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>-processing model.<br />

They maintained that <strong>the</strong>re was a fast, str<strong>on</strong>g inhibiti<strong>on</strong> plus<br />

slower facilitati<strong>on</strong> and inhibiti<strong>on</strong> deriving from resp<strong>on</strong>se bias.<br />

Glaser and Diingelh<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>f (1984) varied SOA in <strong>the</strong> picture-<br />

word task. The pattern was very similar to that in <strong>the</strong> color-<br />

word task: Word-reading time was unaffected by SOAs <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> up to<br />

400 ms in ei<strong>the</strong>r directi<strong>on</strong>, whereas picture naming showed a<br />

str<strong>on</strong>g influence. Again, when <strong>the</strong> word appeared within 100<br />

ms <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> picture, both interference and facilitati<strong>on</strong> were evi-<br />

dent in picture naming. When <strong>the</strong> word appeared more than<br />

100 ms before <strong>the</strong> picture, <strong>the</strong>re was c<strong>on</strong>siderable facilitati<strong>on</strong><br />

but little interference, as Thomas (1977) reported for <strong>the</strong> color-<br />

word task. Differing patterns <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> facilitati<strong>on</strong> and interference in<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se to SOA manipulati<strong>on</strong> have also been reported in <strong>the</strong><br />

flanker task (Grice, Boroughs, & Canham, 1984; Grice &<br />

Gwynne, 1985).<br />

Basically, <strong>the</strong> SOA pattern seems quite c<strong>on</strong>sistent in both <strong>the</strong><br />

color-word and picture-word tasks. This permits <strong>the</strong> following<br />

generalizati<strong>on</strong> to be made: When <strong>the</strong> color (or picture) is to be<br />

named, maximal impact <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a c<strong>on</strong>gruent or inc<strong>on</strong>gruent word will<br />

be observed when <strong>the</strong> two dimensi<strong>on</strong>s begin within 100 ms <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> each<br />

o<strong>the</strong>r. Facilitati<strong>on</strong> may extend to l<strong>on</strong>ger SOAs than interference<br />

when <strong>the</strong> word comes first. Manipulating SOA has virtually no<br />

impact <strong>on</strong> word reading unless a very high proporti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>-<br />

gruent trials biases use <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> color to initiate resp<strong>on</strong>se producti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

The major significance <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> SOA studies is that <strong>the</strong>y call into<br />

questi<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> idea that interference is due simply to words being<br />

processed faster than ink colors. If that were true, <strong>the</strong>re ought to<br />

be a point at which an ink color presented sufficiently before a<br />

word would cause reverse <strong>Stroop</strong> interference (and facilitati<strong>on</strong>).<br />

C<strong>on</strong>certed empirical efforts have failed to find any evidence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

such a point.<br />

The Reverse <strong>Stroop</strong> <strong>Effect</strong><br />

If a word normally interferes with naming a color or a pic-<br />

ture, but a situati<strong>on</strong> is created wherein <strong>the</strong> color or picture inter-


feres with reading <strong>the</strong> word, this is an instance <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a reverse<br />

<strong>Stroop</strong> effect. Such findings are <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>oretical interest because<br />

<strong>the</strong>y relate to <strong>the</strong> idea that interference depends <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> relative<br />

speed <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> each <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> two dimensi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

The first report <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a reverse <strong>Stroop</strong> effect was by <strong>Stroop</strong><br />

(1935b, Experiment 3) himself in <strong>the</strong> seminal article. This effect<br />

arose <strong>on</strong>ly after c<strong>on</strong>siderable training and was quite transient.<br />

Since <strong>the</strong>n, o<strong>the</strong>rs have been more successful in creating stable<br />

reverse <strong>Stroop</strong> situati<strong>on</strong>s, but <strong>the</strong>ir success has usually relied <strong>on</strong><br />

substantially changing <strong>the</strong> task, making it difficult to compare<br />

<strong>the</strong>ir results to <strong>the</strong> standard <strong>Stroop</strong> situati<strong>on</strong>. Still <strong>the</strong> existence<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> reverse <strong>Stroop</strong> effect is now widely accepted.<br />

As an illustrati<strong>on</strong>, Palef and Ols<strong>on</strong> (1975; see also Seymour,<br />

1973) had subjects resp<strong>on</strong>d ei<strong>the</strong>r to <strong>the</strong> meanings <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> words<br />

above and below or to <strong>the</strong>ir spatial locati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> screen. In<br />

Experiment 1, spatial positi<strong>on</strong> was made easier (and hence<br />

faster) to process than word meaning. In this case, inc<strong>on</strong>gruent<br />

spatial positi<strong>on</strong> interfered with a keypress decisi<strong>on</strong> about word<br />

meaning, but inc<strong>on</strong>gruent word meaning did not interfere with<br />

a keypress decisi<strong>on</strong> about spatial positi<strong>on</strong>. In Experiment 2,<br />

where spatial positi<strong>on</strong> was made harder to process, <strong>the</strong> pattern<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interference reversed. In general, <strong>the</strong> faster dimensi<strong>on</strong> inter-<br />

fered with <strong>the</strong> slower <strong>on</strong>e but not vice versa.<br />

Quite a number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> o<strong>the</strong>r studies have dem<strong>on</strong>strated reverse<br />

<strong>Stroop</strong> effects as well (e.g., Abramczyk, Jordan, & Hegel, 1983;<br />

Chen & Ho, 1986; Chmiel, 1984; Dunbar & MacLeod, 1984;<br />

Francolini & Egeth, 1980; Glaser & Dolt, 1977; MacLeod &<br />

Dunbar, 1988; Martin, 1981; Morikawa, 1981; Nealis, 1974;<br />

Pritchatt, 1968; Shor, 1975; Warren & Marsh, 1979). Many <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

<strong>the</strong>se studies used n<strong>on</strong>verbal resp<strong>on</strong>ses, although it is not clear<br />

how crucial resp<strong>on</strong>se mode was because <strong>the</strong> stimuli generally<br />

were ra<strong>the</strong>r different from <strong>the</strong> basic <strong>Stroop</strong> task as well.<br />

The two studies generally cited as illustrative <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reverse<br />

<strong>Stroop</strong> interference are those <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Gumenik and Glass (1970) and<br />

Dyer and Severance (1972). Both involved reducing <strong>the</strong> readabil-<br />

ity <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> words by partial obliterati<strong>on</strong>. Both found a reverse<br />

<strong>Stroop</strong> effect in that ink colors influenced word reading for<br />

hard-to-read words. Unfortunately, like many <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>rs in<br />

<strong>the</strong> list, <strong>the</strong>se two did not examine interference in both "direc-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong>s" in <strong>the</strong> same experiment.<br />

It seems possible, <strong>the</strong>n, to have interference occur from di-<br />

mensi<strong>on</strong> A <strong>on</strong> dimensi<strong>on</strong> B and vice versa. However, many <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

<strong>the</strong>se experiments are quite far removed from <strong>the</strong> standard<br />

task. Cases where exactly <strong>the</strong> same stimuli and resp<strong>on</strong>ses were<br />

used with <strong>on</strong>ly an instructi<strong>on</strong>al difference are rare, leaving<br />

room for more work in this area. Yet interference can occur in<br />

both directi<strong>on</strong>s when <strong>the</strong> <strong>on</strong>ly change is from naming colors to<br />

reading words (e.g., Dunbar & MacLeod, 1984), c<strong>on</strong>flicting<br />

with <strong>the</strong> belief that <strong>the</strong> faster process interferes with <strong>the</strong> slower<br />

<strong>on</strong>e, and not vice versa. For this reas<strong>on</strong>, <strong>the</strong> following c<strong>on</strong>clu-<br />

si<strong>on</strong> is added to <strong>the</strong> list, although it must remain a little vague at<br />

this point: A reverse <strong>Stroop</strong> effect (i.e., interference with word read-<br />

ing caused by an incompatible, irrelevant ink color) appears to be<br />

possible, but this effect is not simply a c<strong>on</strong>sequence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> relative<br />

speeds <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing each dimensi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Practice<br />

<strong>An</strong> obvious and <strong>the</strong>oretically critical questi<strong>on</strong> is: Does <strong>the</strong><br />

<strong>Stroop</strong> effect change as a c<strong>on</strong>sequence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> (type <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>) practice at<br />

THE STROOP EFFECT 181<br />

<strong>the</strong> task? If so, how does it change, and what does this tell us<br />

about <strong>the</strong> cause <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> phenomen<strong>on</strong>? <strong>Stroop</strong> and his predeces-<br />

sors realized <strong>the</strong> importance <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> this factor; now I will examine<br />

what has been found out since <strong>the</strong>n in studies manipulating<br />

practice.<br />

On <strong>the</strong> view that words interfere with color naming because<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> our extensive reading experience, <strong>Stroop</strong> interference is a<br />

direct c<strong>on</strong>sequence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> differential practice. Thus, variati<strong>on</strong> in<br />

practice should have direct impact <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> task. <strong>Stroop</strong> (1935b,<br />

Experiment 3) recognized this, although he thought that c<strong>on</strong>sid-<br />

erable practice might be needed to modify existing resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

tendencies. It is unfortunate, <strong>the</strong>n, that <strong>the</strong> relevant studies <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>-<br />

ten involve very little practice (e.g., Alpers<strong>on</strong>, 1967, used <strong>the</strong><br />

range <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 3 to 50 trials). It is hardly surprising that such studies<br />

c<strong>on</strong>clude that practice is not a particularly critical variable. Per-<br />

haps <strong>the</strong> Jensen and Rohwer (1966) claim that almost all prac-<br />

tice effects occur within <strong>the</strong> first 5 trials is resp<strong>on</strong>sible for this. I<br />

believe <strong>the</strong>y were wr<strong>on</strong>g.<br />

Intuitively, extended practice with <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> task should lead<br />

to reduced interference as subjects develop a strategy for coping<br />

more successfully with <strong>the</strong> task. This result has indeed been<br />

observed (e.g., Etfler, 1978a; Ogura, 1980), but not always (e.g.,<br />

Harbes<strong>on</strong> et al., 1982; Shor et al., 1972; White, 1978). Fur<strong>the</strong>r-<br />

more, this is not <strong>the</strong> whole sto~ As <strong>on</strong>e illustrati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> how<br />

much more complex <strong>the</strong> situati<strong>on</strong> is, <strong>the</strong> stimuli and resp<strong>on</strong>ses<br />

chosen are also important in training studies. Flowers and<br />

Stoup (I 977) found that interference dissipated with practice for<br />

n<strong>on</strong>integrated stimuli when <strong>the</strong> task was sorting, but not when<br />

it was oral naming. Nielsen (1975) found initially greater inter-<br />

ference for a vocal than for a manual resp<strong>on</strong>se, but <strong>the</strong>n found a<br />

greater decline in interference over practice for <strong>the</strong> manual than<br />

for <strong>the</strong> vocal resp<strong>on</strong>se. The results <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Roe, Wils<strong>on</strong>craft, and<br />

Griffiths (1980) c<strong>on</strong>firm that manual resp<strong>on</strong>ding is affected<br />

more quickly by practice, which is interesting given that manual<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ding is faster to begin with and <strong>the</strong>refore closer to a "per-<br />

formance floor."<br />

It also seems that <strong>the</strong>re is a good deal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> specificity to practice<br />

effects. M6nard-Buteau and Cavanagh (1984) found that, for<br />

items like "blue banana," <strong>the</strong>re was interference in saying<br />

"blue" to <strong>the</strong> word or <strong>the</strong> picture in color. However, extended<br />

practice <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> word versi<strong>on</strong> did not transfer to <strong>the</strong> picture<br />

versi<strong>on</strong>. They argued that <strong>the</strong> semantic element <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interference<br />

did not coincide in <strong>the</strong> two tasks, and that interference might be<br />

more at <strong>the</strong> encoding stage for pictures and <strong>the</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>se stage<br />

for words. This seems worth pursuing, particularly because it<br />

suggests that <strong>the</strong> comm<strong>on</strong> assumpti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> process identity in<br />

<strong>the</strong>se two tasks may be at least partly incorrect.<br />

In c<strong>on</strong>trast to M6nard-Buteau and Cavanagh (1984), Reis-<br />

berg, Bar<strong>on</strong>, and Kemler (1980) did obtain transfer al<strong>on</strong>g se-<br />

mantic lines. They used <strong>the</strong> counting task where <strong>the</strong> items to be<br />

counted are <strong>the</strong>mselves digits. With practice, interference de-<br />

clined but in a very specific way. Subjects who had practiced<br />

with <strong>the</strong> digits 2 and 4 to be ignored showed no benefit <strong>on</strong> a<br />

counting test where <strong>the</strong> digits I and 3 or <strong>the</strong> words to and for<br />

were to be ignored. However, <strong>the</strong>re was good transfer when <strong>the</strong><br />

words two and four were to be ignored. Thus, meaning appears<br />

to be involved.<br />

Regan (1977) compared a well-practiced to an unpracticed<br />

task. She had English-speaking subjects learn <strong>the</strong> names <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Ar-


182 COLIN M. MACLEOD<br />

menian letters. She <strong>the</strong>n composed large English and Arme-<br />

nian letters out <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> small English and Armenian letters, and re-<br />

quired subjects to name <strong>the</strong> small letters while ignoring <strong>the</strong><br />

large letters, using <strong>the</strong> Nav<strong>on</strong> (1977) procedure. Interference<br />

was equivalent for <strong>the</strong> very familiar set and for <strong>the</strong> newly<br />

learned set. This result speaks to what is meant by "automatic"<br />

processing: Although such processing appears to be involun-<br />

tary, it apparently does not require extensive practice.<br />

Reas<strong>on</strong>ing that a novel orientati<strong>on</strong> would slow down word<br />

processing, Liu (1973) had subjects rotate <strong>the</strong>ir <strong>Stroop</strong> cards<br />

upside down and <strong>the</strong>n name <strong>the</strong> ink colors. Interference was<br />

reduced compared with normally oriented words. However, us-<br />

ing several unique word orientati<strong>on</strong>s, Dunbar and MacLeod<br />

(1984) failed to c<strong>on</strong>firm Liu's result. In fact, we found equiva-<br />

lent interference regardless <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> word's orientati<strong>on</strong>, so that<br />

previous practice differences could not be <strong>the</strong> critical factor.<br />

Most recently, MacLeod and Dunbar (1988) took <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> prac-<br />

tice issue directly. We had subjects learn to call each <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> four<br />

unique shapes by a different color name (green, pink, orange, or<br />

blue). We could <strong>the</strong>n present each shape in a neutral color<br />

(white), a c<strong>on</strong>gruent color, or an inc<strong>on</strong>gruent color, and require<br />

subjects to name <strong>the</strong> shape or <strong>the</strong> ink color. With a small<br />

amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> shape-naming practice, ink color interfered with<br />

shape naming but shape name did not interfere with ink-color<br />

naming. With moderate practice, interference was equivalent in<br />

both directi<strong>on</strong>s. With extensive practice, <strong>on</strong>ly shape names in-<br />

terfered with ink-color naming and not vice versa. Practice had<br />

a systematic impact, interestingly in a task with no "word" di-<br />

mensi<strong>on</strong> at all and where shape naming never became faster<br />

than ink-color naming.<br />

Practice warrants fur<strong>the</strong>r investigati<strong>on</strong>. Al<strong>on</strong>g <strong>the</strong> way, vari-<br />

ables such as intertrial interval (see Doten, 1955) could also be<br />

examined. In our work (MacLeod & Dunbar, 1988), we manipu-<br />

lated practice <strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong>e dimensi<strong>on</strong>, leaving <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r in its "natu-<br />

ral state"; it would be informative to covary practice <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> two<br />

dimensi<strong>on</strong>s, a project that Diane Williams and I have under<br />

way in my laboratory. For <strong>the</strong> present, my general c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong><br />

about practice is probably too general: Degree <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> practice in<br />

processing each <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> dimensi<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a multidimensi<strong>on</strong>al stimu-<br />

lus is influential in determining <strong>the</strong> extent <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interference from <strong>on</strong>e<br />

dimensi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> ano<strong>the</strong>r. The greater <strong>the</strong> practice in processing a<br />

dimensi<strong>on</strong>, <strong>the</strong> more capable that dimensi<strong>on</strong> is <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> influencing <strong>the</strong><br />

processing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> ano<strong>the</strong>r dimensi<strong>on</strong>. Practice may turn out to be<br />

<strong>on</strong>e <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> most effective manipulati<strong>on</strong>s for disentangling <strong>the</strong>-<br />

ories <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> effect.<br />

This ends <strong>the</strong> secti<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> manipulati<strong>on</strong>s over multiple trials.<br />

Of course, <strong>the</strong>re are isolated articles examining more esoteric<br />

effects. One example is studies in which feedback is manipu-<br />

lated, as when vocal feedback is minimized (Breslow, Grand, &<br />

Freedman, 1980; Grand, Breslow, & Freedman, 1980) or de-<br />

layed (Doehrman, Landau, & O~<strong>on</strong>nell, 1978) or when re-<br />

sp<strong>on</strong>se feedback is varied in some o<strong>the</strong>r way (e.g., Hochman,<br />

1973). <strong>An</strong>o<strong>the</strong>r is <strong>the</strong> case <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> manipulati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> effort in <strong>the</strong> task<br />

(e.g., MacKinn<strong>on</strong>, Geiselman, & Woodward, 1985). However,<br />

<strong>the</strong> main findings are those outlined previously. I now shift <strong>the</strong><br />

focus from stimulus manipulati<strong>on</strong>s to those c<strong>on</strong>centrating <strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>se side <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> task.<br />

Resp<strong>on</strong>se-Related Manipulati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

Under <strong>the</strong> view that interference derives from a logjam at a<br />

limited-capacity resp<strong>on</strong>se buffer because <strong>the</strong> irrelevant word<br />

has entered before <strong>the</strong> relevant ink color (e.g., Mort<strong>on</strong>, 1969;<br />

Mort<strong>on</strong> & Chambers, 1973), <strong>the</strong> locus <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interference is placed<br />

squarely in <strong>the</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>se stage. Thus, manipulati<strong>on</strong>s affecting<br />

how or when <strong>the</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>se is made, or how many resp<strong>on</strong>ses<br />

might possibly be made, become very important in testing such<br />

a view.<br />

Order <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

Few studies have required subjects to make two resp<strong>on</strong>ses to<br />

<strong>the</strong> same stimulus as, for example, when c<strong>on</strong>secutive resp<strong>on</strong>ses<br />

to <strong>the</strong> ink color and <strong>the</strong> word (or vice versa) are required. The<br />

prototype work in this domain is that <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Schweickert (1978,<br />

1983), who argued that <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e decisi<strong>on</strong> can be made at a time<br />

in <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> task. As illustrati<strong>on</strong>s, he cited several studies (e.g.,<br />

Ghols<strong>on</strong> & Hohle, 1968a; Hock & Egeth, 1970; Rieck & Coates,<br />

1977), all involving <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e resp<strong>on</strong>se. To dem<strong>on</strong>strate that at<br />

least some <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> processing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> word and <strong>the</strong> ink color is<br />

c<strong>on</strong>current, he cited Greenwald (1972). Here subjects saw an<br />

arrow pointing left or right and heard <strong>the</strong> word left or right<br />

simultaneously. They had to resp<strong>on</strong>d orally to <strong>on</strong>e dimensi<strong>on</strong><br />

and manually to <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r. Greenwald varied whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>on</strong>e or<br />

both arrows or words could occur in a block <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> trials, and<br />

whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong>re was c<strong>on</strong>flict between <strong>the</strong> dimensi<strong>on</strong>s. His data<br />

showed clear evidence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interacti<strong>on</strong> am<strong>on</strong>g <strong>the</strong>se variables.<br />

Schweickert's (1983) experiment used a colored rectangle<br />

above a color word. There were 2 subjects. One was to press a<br />

key indicating <strong>the</strong> color <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> rectangle before reading <strong>the</strong><br />

word aloud; her data suggested that she decided about <strong>the</strong> ink<br />

color first. The o<strong>the</strong>r subject was to press a key for <strong>the</strong> word and<br />

<strong>the</strong>n name <strong>the</strong> ink color aloud; her data suggested that she<br />

decided about <strong>the</strong> word first. This outcome is difficult to han-<br />

dle under a differential speed-<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>-processing account, which ex-<br />

plains <strong>the</strong> usual asymmetry as being due to <strong>the</strong> decisi<strong>on</strong> about<br />

<strong>the</strong> word preceding <strong>the</strong> decisi<strong>on</strong> about <strong>the</strong> ink color. This ap-<br />

pears to be true when <strong>the</strong> subject names <strong>the</strong> ink color. However,<br />

<strong>the</strong> decisi<strong>on</strong> order is reversed when <strong>the</strong> word must be read, so<br />

why is <strong>the</strong>re an asymmetry in <strong>the</strong> standard <strong>Stroop</strong> task?<br />

Work involving two resp<strong>on</strong>ses (see also Virzi & Egeth, 1985,<br />

Experiment 4) may help to explain how <strong>the</strong> various processes<br />

are scheduled. Of course, as Schweickert was well aware, <strong>the</strong><br />

danger is that scheduling is influenced by whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>on</strong>e or more<br />

than <strong>on</strong>e resp<strong>on</strong>se is required. This may also be reflected in<br />

memory. Corballis and Lu<strong>the</strong> (1971) presented subjects with<br />

three inc<strong>on</strong>gruent <strong>Stroop</strong> items in rapid successi<strong>on</strong>, and varied<br />

whe<strong>the</strong>r subjects had to recall <strong>on</strong> an item by item basis (i.e., <strong>the</strong><br />

ink color and word for each item) or dimensi<strong>on</strong>ally (i.e., all three<br />

ink colors and <strong>the</strong>n all three words). When words were reported<br />

before colors, dimensi<strong>on</strong>al recall was better than item-based<br />

recall. When colors were reported before words, <strong>the</strong> pattern<br />

reversed. Again scheduling <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processes apparently was driven<br />

by task demands. We need to know more about how sequencing<br />

is governed.<br />

Resp<strong>on</strong>se Modality: Oral Versus Manual<br />

If interference occurs at or near resp<strong>on</strong>se output, it seems<br />

reas<strong>on</strong>able to examine <strong>the</strong> form <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> resp<strong>on</strong>se. The critical c<strong>on</strong>-<br />

trast has been between vocal-resp<strong>on</strong>se output--naming--and<br />

manual-resp<strong>on</strong>se output--a keypress. The questi<strong>on</strong> is whe<strong>the</strong>r


interference differs over mode <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> resp<strong>on</strong>se in <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> task. In<br />

particular, does <strong>the</strong> compatibility <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> verbal resp<strong>on</strong>se and<br />

<strong>the</strong> irrelevant word account for some or all <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> interference,<br />

as studies such as those <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Ghols<strong>on</strong> and Hohle (1968b) and<br />

Belier (1975) might suggest?<br />

Pritchatt (1968) suggested that <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> effect was a c<strong>on</strong>se-<br />

quence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> verbal output because <strong>the</strong> effect was reduced when a<br />

matching resp<strong>on</strong>se was used. However, White (1969) was <strong>the</strong><br />

first to make <strong>the</strong> explicit comparis<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> verbal and n<strong>on</strong>verbal<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ses. For <strong>the</strong> standard <strong>Stroop</strong> task, he reported less inter-<br />

ference with <strong>the</strong> n<strong>on</strong>verbal keypress resp<strong>on</strong>se than with <strong>the</strong> oral<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se. Unfortunately, he used as his dependent variable <strong>the</strong><br />

ratio <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> inc<strong>on</strong>gruent time to c<strong>on</strong>trol time, so it is difficult to tell<br />

exactly what <strong>the</strong> pattern looked like in <strong>the</strong> individual c<strong>on</strong>di-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

One <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> best known resp<strong>on</strong>se-related studies was by Keele<br />

(1972), although he used <strong>on</strong>ly manual resp<strong>on</strong>ses. He reas<strong>on</strong>ed<br />

that if interference arises during memory retrieval, <strong>the</strong>n any<br />

word should interfere with naming ink color be it a n<strong>on</strong>color<br />

word or a color word. In c<strong>on</strong>trast, if n<strong>on</strong>color words do not<br />

interfere but color words do, <strong>the</strong>n <strong>the</strong> retrieval stage cannot be<br />

<strong>the</strong> locus <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interference. His results supported <strong>the</strong> latter predic-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong>, leading him (and Posner & Snyder, 1975) to a late-selecti<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong>ory <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> attenti<strong>on</strong>. In fact, though, <strong>the</strong> Keele (1972) study does<br />

not directly address <strong>the</strong> issue <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> resp<strong>on</strong>se modality; his study is<br />

more a replicati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Klein's (1964) logic using keypresses in-<br />

stead <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> oral resp<strong>on</strong>ses. 8<br />

More crucial are studies where <strong>the</strong> two resp<strong>on</strong>se modes are<br />

directly compared in <strong>the</strong> same experiment. Nielsen (1974) var-<br />

ied <strong>the</strong> irrelevant stimulus from c<strong>on</strong>trol Xs to color words,<br />

roughly following Klein (1964). Interference much like Klein's<br />

was observed for both vocal and keypress resp<strong>on</strong>ses, although<br />

interference was greater for vocal resp<strong>on</strong>ses. Even with practice,<br />

vocal-resp<strong>on</strong>se interference exceeded manual-resp<strong>on</strong>se interfer-<br />

ence. Using similar logic, Majeres (1974) had subjects ei<strong>the</strong>r say<br />

or write <strong>the</strong>ir resp<strong>on</strong>se (<strong>on</strong>ly <strong>the</strong> first letter had to be written).<br />

There was a clear gradient <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interference for both resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

modes, but less interference when writing than when saying <strong>the</strong><br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se.<br />

Redding and Gerjets (1977) dem<strong>on</strong>strated that inc<strong>on</strong>gruent<br />

words produced 177 ms <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interference when <strong>the</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>se to<br />

ink color was oral and <strong>on</strong>ly 98 ms when <strong>the</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>se was man-<br />

ual; c<strong>on</strong>gruent words produced 23 ms <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> facilitati<strong>on</strong> for oral<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ses and 67 ms for manual resp<strong>on</strong>ses. Curiously, <strong>the</strong>re<br />

appeared to be less interference but more facilitati<strong>on</strong> when <strong>the</strong><br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se was manual compared with oral. Whe<strong>the</strong>r this general-<br />

izes remains to be seen.<br />

Perhaps <strong>the</strong> pivotal questi<strong>on</strong> is whe<strong>the</strong>r interference c<strong>on</strong>sis-<br />

tently appears when resp<strong>on</strong>ding is manual. In general, <strong>the</strong> an-<br />

swer appears to be "yes" Several articles have c<strong>on</strong>firmed inter-<br />

ference with a manual resp<strong>on</strong>se (e.g., Logan et al., 1984; Roe et<br />

al., 1980; Schmit & Davis, 1974; Virzi & Egeth, 1985; Warren &<br />

Marsh, 1979). Some, such as that <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Roe et al. (I 980), even found<br />

no overall differences between <strong>the</strong> two resp<strong>on</strong>se modes. The<br />

<strong>on</strong>ly case where <strong>the</strong>re ~vas no interference with a manual re-<br />

sp<strong>on</strong>se appears to be <strong>the</strong> experiment by McClain (1983b) de-<br />

scribed shortly.<br />

A feature <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> any task that c<strong>on</strong>tributes to its difficulty is <strong>the</strong><br />

degree to which <strong>the</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>ses map readily <strong>on</strong>to <strong>the</strong> various<br />

dimensi<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> stimulus. Fitts and Posner (1967) referred to<br />

THE STROOP EFFECT 183<br />

this as <strong>the</strong> stimulus-resp<strong>on</strong>se compatibility issue. Treisman<br />

and Fearnley (1969) stressed this in <strong>the</strong>ir account <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong><br />

effect. Put simply, does <strong>the</strong> word interfere with naming <strong>the</strong> color<br />

because <strong>the</strong> verbal nature <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> word matches that <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> re-<br />

sp<strong>on</strong>se?<br />

McClain manipulated compatibility in both <strong>the</strong> standard<br />

<strong>Stroop</strong> task and its auditory analog. In <strong>the</strong> color-word task<br />

(McClain, 1983b), interference in color naming declined as <strong>the</strong><br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se moved from oral to pressing butt<strong>on</strong>s labeled with<br />

color names to pressing butt<strong>on</strong>s labeled with color patches. In<br />

fact, interference was not significant in <strong>the</strong> last c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, but<br />

such complete absence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interference is an unusual result. 9 In<br />

<strong>the</strong> auditory experiment (McClain, 1983c), subjects had to<br />

identify ei<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> pitch (high or low) or <strong>the</strong> word (high or low).<br />

When pitch was to be identified, interference occurred for an<br />

oral or a keypress resp<strong>on</strong>se, but not for a hummed resp<strong>on</strong>se.<br />

When <strong>the</strong> word was to be identified, <strong>the</strong> pattern reversed.<br />

Although Mort<strong>on</strong> and Chambers (1973) questi<strong>on</strong>ed <strong>the</strong> role<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> stimulus-resp<strong>on</strong>se compatibility, it seems to matter (Flowers<br />

et al., 1979; Sim<strong>on</strong> & Sudalaimuthu, 1979; Zakay & Glicksohn,<br />

1985). Of course, comparing resp<strong>on</strong>se times across oral and<br />

manual resp<strong>on</strong>ses can be a thorny interpretive problem (see,<br />

e.g., Stanovich & Pachella, 1977). Recall that Neill (1977)<br />

showed that if <strong>the</strong> irrelevant word <strong>on</strong> trial n - 1 named <strong>the</strong><br />

relevant ink color <strong>on</strong> trial n, this slowed oral resp<strong>on</strong>ding (rela-<br />

tive to unrelated items). Yet when resp<strong>on</strong>se modality was man-<br />

ual, <strong>the</strong> same sequential relati<strong>on</strong> actually speeded resp<strong>on</strong>ding.<br />

Why? Neill argued that suppressi<strong>on</strong> occurred <strong>on</strong>ly when <strong>the</strong><br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se had to be spoken, whereas activati<strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong> manual<br />

task helped eliminate <strong>the</strong> word faster from c<strong>on</strong>tenti<strong>on</strong> as a possi-<br />

ble resp<strong>on</strong>se. Sim<strong>on</strong> and Sudalaimuthu (1979) reported a re-<br />

lated effect obtained within single trials.<br />

Such results are puzzling. Never<strong>the</strong>less, <strong>the</strong>re is sufficient<br />

c<strong>on</strong>vergence to permit <strong>the</strong> following c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong>: Although still<br />

significant, interference (but perhaps not facilitati<strong>on</strong>) is reduced<br />

when resp<strong>on</strong>se modality is switched from oral to manual. Stimu-<br />

lus-resp<strong>on</strong>se compatibility matters; if <strong>the</strong> normal processing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

<strong>the</strong> irrelevant dimensi<strong>on</strong> leads to a resp<strong>on</strong>se in <strong>the</strong> mode desig-<br />

nated for <strong>the</strong> relevant dimensi<strong>on</strong>, interference is likely to be<br />

heightened. However, nei<strong>the</strong>r resp<strong>on</strong>se mode al<strong>on</strong>e nor <strong>the</strong> in-<br />

teracti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> stimulus and resp<strong>on</strong>se modes can account for <strong>the</strong><br />

<strong>Stroop</strong> effect. The effect is due to more than a queuing problem<br />

at <strong>the</strong> finish line.<br />

Resp<strong>on</strong>se Set Size and Compositi<strong>on</strong><br />

It frequently is argued that people read <strong>the</strong> irrelevant word in<br />

<strong>Stroop</strong> variati<strong>on</strong>s because it usually is an eligible resp<strong>on</strong>se (e.g.,<br />

Klein, 1964; Proctor, 1978; see Stimulus Set Size). If interfer-<br />

8 It should be noted that Keele (1972) did report substantial interfer-<br />

ence when comparing inc<strong>on</strong>gruent stimuli to c<strong>on</strong>trol stimuli despite<br />

<strong>the</strong> manual resp<strong>on</strong>se mode.<br />

9 McClain used <strong>the</strong> difference between inc<strong>on</strong>gruent and c<strong>on</strong>gruent<br />

times as her measure <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interference, which unfortunately combines<br />

interference and facilitati<strong>on</strong>. Wheeler (1977) found no difference be-<br />

tween color naming and word reading in <strong>the</strong> inc<strong>on</strong>gruent c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong><br />

using a manual resp<strong>on</strong>se, but this is a very unc<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al way to<br />

c<strong>on</strong>trast resp<strong>on</strong>se modes.


184 COLIN M. MACLEOD<br />

ence occurs in resp<strong>on</strong>ding, <strong>the</strong>n manipulati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> set <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ses might be expected to have some effect.<br />

Both Klein (1964) and Scheibe et al. (1967) reported twice as<br />

much interference when <strong>the</strong> color word was <strong>the</strong> name <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> an-<br />

o<strong>the</strong>r possible ink color in <strong>the</strong> set. Even c<strong>on</strong>trolling for word<br />

frequency and associati<strong>on</strong>, Proctor (1978) still found 24 to 30<br />

ms more interference when <strong>the</strong> words and ink colors coincided.<br />

O<strong>the</strong>r studies support this c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong> color-word task<br />

(e.g., Stifling, 1979) and in <strong>the</strong> picture-word task (Lupker &<br />

Katz, 1981). Lupker and Katz found more interference when<br />

irrelevant words named <strong>the</strong> categories <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> possible picture<br />

names than when <strong>the</strong>y did not. Dunbar (1986) provided an<br />

extensive discussi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> relevant picture-word literature,<br />

and La Heij (1988) reported careful studies showing that re-<br />

sp<strong>on</strong>se-set members produce heightened interference. Studies<br />

using <strong>the</strong> flanker task (e.g., La Heij et al., 1985) also found that a<br />

target word suffers more when a nearby word is a potential<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se than when it is not.<br />

There are two logical possibilities for what should happen as<br />

set size increases. First, interference might increase because<br />

more potential resp<strong>on</strong>ses are vying for output, making manag-<br />

ing <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tents <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> working memory more effortful. Sec<strong>on</strong>d,<br />

interference might decrease because <strong>the</strong> probability that any<br />

particular resp<strong>on</strong>se is actually in working memory would de-<br />

crease with larger resp<strong>on</strong>se set size. Nielsen (1974) found that<br />

interference increased with increasing set size, Ray (1974)<br />

found that it was unaffected, and La Heij et al. (1985) found that<br />

it decreased. There are o<strong>the</strong>r studies that could be aligned with<br />

each <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>se findings (see also Stimulus Set Size). The manipu-<br />

lati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> resp<strong>on</strong>se set size seems so straightforward that <strong>the</strong>se<br />

c<strong>on</strong>flicting findings are frustrating. Thus, <strong>the</strong> recent emphasis<br />

<strong>on</strong> this issue in <strong>the</strong> work <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>La Heij and colleagues (e.g., La Heij,<br />

1988; La Heij et al., 1985) is timely.<br />

The findings <strong>on</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>se set are clear with respect to <strong>the</strong><br />

overlap <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> two dimensi<strong>on</strong>s, but not with respect to <strong>the</strong> effect<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> set-size variati<strong>on</strong>. This is captured in <strong>the</strong> following c<strong>on</strong>clu-<br />

si<strong>on</strong>: When <strong>the</strong> irrelevant dimensi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a set <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> stimufi includes<br />

names that are eligible resp<strong>on</strong>ses for <strong>the</strong> relevant dimensi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

more interference results than when <strong>the</strong> sets are n<strong>on</strong>overlapping<br />

Although variati<strong>on</strong>s in resp<strong>on</strong>se set size might be expected to<br />

affect interference, existing results are unclear.<br />

Individual Differences<br />

A great many articles have examined <strong>the</strong> relati<strong>on</strong> between<br />

<strong>Stroop</strong> interference and some individual differences parameter.<br />

The purpose <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> most <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>se clearly is not to explain <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong><br />

effect, so for this reas<strong>on</strong> I omit many <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>se studies. Instead, I<br />

select <strong>on</strong>ly a few dimensi<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> individual variati<strong>on</strong> to review<br />

here. As Kareev (1982) illustrated, certain analyses at <strong>the</strong> level<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> individual have c<strong>on</strong>siderable potential in helping us to<br />

understand <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> effect.<br />

Sex Differences<br />

The most obvious difference between individuals is sex, so<br />

much effort has been directed to exploring sex differences (<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>.<br />

Maccoby & Jacklin, 1974). The <strong>Stroop</strong> task has not escaped:<br />

There are now over a dozen studies <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> how men and women<br />

differ in degree <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interference. Even before <strong>Stroop</strong>, Lig<strong>on</strong><br />

(1932; see also Brown, 1915) noted that girls named colors faster<br />

than did boys, although <strong>the</strong>re was no difference in word-read-<br />

ing speed. <strong>Stroop</strong> (1935b) c<strong>on</strong>firmed this, but critically showed<br />

that men and women did not display differential interference.<br />

This result has been replicated <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>ten (e.g., Golden, 1974b; Sar-<br />

manay, 1977; Sladekova & Daniel, 1981), yet investigators still<br />

pursue <strong>the</strong> matter (e.g., Peretti, 1969, 1971).<br />

Naish (1980) suggested that men may encode irrelevant<br />

words both orthographically and ph<strong>on</strong>emically when sorting<br />

<strong>the</strong> colors in which <strong>the</strong>y are written, whereas women encode<br />

<strong>on</strong>ly ph<strong>on</strong>emically. However, he also suggested that this strat-<br />

egy difference might be unique to certain <strong>Stroop</strong>-like situati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

and not evident in normal reading.<br />

Several studies have examined <strong>the</strong> relati<strong>on</strong> between <strong>Stroop</strong><br />

interference and some test <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interest, with sex differences as a<br />

subsidiary analysis. In a factor-analytic study <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> extraversi<strong>on</strong><br />

and field dependence, loadings <strong>on</strong> a <strong>Stroop</strong> factor were higher<br />

for men than for women (B<strong>on</strong>e & Eysenck, 1972). In a study <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

<strong>the</strong> relati<strong>on</strong> between a test <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> matching familiar figures and <strong>the</strong><br />

<strong>Stroop</strong> test (Boyden & Gilpin, 1978), errors <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> two tests<br />

were positively correlated for men but not for women, which <strong>the</strong><br />

authors took as an indicati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> distractibility and impulsivity<br />

being related in men.<br />

Jorgens<strong>on</strong> and her colleagues (Davis, Jorgens<strong>on</strong>, Kritselis, &<br />

Opella, 1981; Davis, Jorgens<strong>on</strong>, & Opella, 1983; Jorgens<strong>on</strong>, Da-<br />

vis, Opella, & <strong>An</strong>gerstein, 1980, 1981; Jorgens<strong>on</strong>, Davis, Wil-<br />

b<strong>on</strong>, & Opella, 1983) have been especially interested in hemi-<br />

spheric differences and sex differences in <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> task. In<br />

brief, sex differences in both grade-school and college students<br />

did not appear to account for much <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> variance in <strong>Stroop</strong><br />

performance in <strong>the</strong>ir studies.<br />

In grade-school children, girls have been shown to be gener-<br />

ally faster but not better at color recogniti<strong>on</strong> and not different in<br />

terms <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interference, <strong>the</strong> most interesting measure <strong>the</strong>oreti-<br />

cally (Dash & Dash, 1982). Bettner, Jarvik, and Blum (1971)<br />

studied an aged populati<strong>on</strong> and found poorer <strong>Stroop</strong> perfor-<br />

mance in men than in women (average age <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 84 years).<br />

On <strong>the</strong> whole, research has failed to find much difference in<br />

<strong>Stroop</strong> interference between men and women at any age. Al-<br />

though women may be somewhat faster, especially in naming<br />

colors, this relates to general resp<strong>on</strong>se speed (see Jensen, 1965),<br />

not to <strong>the</strong> derived measure <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interference. Here is a summary<br />

statement: There are no sex differences in <strong>Stroop</strong> interference at<br />

any age. Perhaps this is too str<strong>on</strong>g, but I remain to be c<strong>on</strong>-<br />

vinced.<br />

Age Differences<br />

The development <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> color and word-form processing with<br />

age was <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interest before <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> task was created (e.g., Brian<br />

& Goodenough, 1929; Lig<strong>on</strong>, 1932). Basically, more recent stud-<br />

ies make similar points. Thus, Cramer (1967) supported Lig<strong>on</strong>'s<br />

claim that form processing dominates over color processing,<br />

although preschoolers may show <strong>the</strong> teverse preference (Aro-<br />

chova, 1971), having not yet learned to recognize letter forms<br />

very well.<br />

Interest in development with regard to <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> task itself<br />

did not arise until <strong>the</strong> mid-1960s. There are some three dozen


pertinent studies, <strong>the</strong> first being a life-span study by Comalli,<br />

Wapner, and Werner (1962; see also Rand et al., 1963). Testing<br />

subjects from ages 7 to 80 years, <strong>the</strong>y observed greatest interfer-<br />

ence in <strong>the</strong> young children; interference declined into adult-<br />

hood and <strong>the</strong>n increased again with advanced age. They saw<br />

young children and older adults as having relatively more diffi-<br />

culty in screening out interfering stimuli. Ehri and Wilce (1979)<br />

produced an experimental analog <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> this result by training first<br />

and sec<strong>on</strong>d graders <strong>on</strong> a set <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>words to be used in <strong>the</strong> picture-<br />

word task. Initially, as <strong>the</strong> subjects were learning to read <strong>the</strong><br />

words, interference increased; however, <strong>on</strong>ce <strong>the</strong> words were<br />

learned and fur<strong>the</strong>r training simply improved resp<strong>on</strong>se speed<br />

to <strong>the</strong> words, interference decreased.<br />

Schiller (1966) helped to clarify <strong>the</strong> n<strong>on</strong>m<strong>on</strong>ot<strong>on</strong>icity in inter-<br />

ference with age in children. Interference was minimal for chil-<br />

dren in Grade 1, younger than <strong>the</strong> youngest subjects in <strong>the</strong><br />

Comalli et al. (1962) study. Interference <strong>the</strong>n became maximal<br />

in Grades 2 and 3 (Comalli et al's youngest subjects), and de-<br />

dined <strong>the</strong>reafter. The str<strong>on</strong>g suggesti<strong>on</strong> is that this early rise<br />

and <strong>the</strong>n fall in interference reflects <strong>the</strong> <strong>on</strong>set <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reading skills.<br />

Dash and Dash (1982) c<strong>on</strong>firmed Schiller's result (see also<br />

Friedman, 1971). Using <strong>the</strong> picture-word task, Ehri (1976)<br />

showed a similar pattern, except that poor readers in <strong>the</strong> sec<strong>on</strong>d<br />

grade did not show interference, behaving ra<strong>the</strong>r like normal<br />

first graders in <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r studies. Attempts to create <strong>Stroop</strong>-like<br />

situati<strong>on</strong>s for younger, prereading children have not been very<br />

informative (e.g., Butollo, Bauer, & Riedl, 1971; Cammock &<br />

Cairns, 1979). Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, attempts to categorize subgroups<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> development in <strong>the</strong> task have not been successful (e.g., Rand<br />

et al., 1963; Silverstein & Franken, 1965).<br />

The decrease in interference through adulthood and before<br />

<strong>the</strong> age <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 60 that Comalli et al. (1962) observed has been c<strong>on</strong>-<br />

firmed (e.g., Wise, Sutt<strong>on</strong>, & Gibb<strong>on</strong>s, 1975), as has <strong>the</strong> increase<br />

in interference for adults older than 60 years (see Cohn, Dust-<br />

man, & Bradford, 1984; Panek, Rush, & Slade, 1984). There<br />

may be excepti<strong>on</strong>s (e.g., Baumler, 1969), but even studies <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

aging twins reveal <strong>the</strong> increased interference with age greater<br />

than 60 years (Bettner et al., 1971; Jarvik, Blum, & Varma,<br />

1972), toge<strong>the</strong>r with <strong>the</strong> suggesti<strong>on</strong> that identical twins show<br />

more similar interference than do fraternal twins. Attempts at<br />

remediati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> exaggerated interference shown by older<br />

adults have not been notably successful (e.g., White, 1978).<br />

Many <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> developmental <strong>Stroop</strong> studies focus <strong>on</strong> reading<br />

skill (e.g., Bakan & Shotland, 1969; Corbitt, 1978; Fournier,<br />

Mazzarella, Ricciardi, & Fingeret, 1975; Frey, 1971; Ludwig &<br />

Lazarus, 1983; West & Stanovich, 1978; Wilder, 1969). The<br />

Corbitt (1978) dissertati<strong>on</strong> provides a good example. He showed<br />

that reading ability and interference were closely related: Inter-<br />

mediate and good readers read words faster than <strong>the</strong>y named<br />

colors, and showed a <strong>Stroop</strong> effect without a reverse <strong>Stroop</strong><br />

effect. Poor readers named colors faster than <strong>the</strong>y could read<br />

words and showed <strong>the</strong> opposite pattern.<br />

How about reading comprehensi<strong>on</strong>? Using <strong>the</strong> picture-word<br />

task, Golink<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>f and Rosinski (1976) showed that good versus<br />

poor comprehenders in Grades 3 and 5 did not differ in <strong>the</strong><br />

degree <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interference shown in naming pictures c<strong>on</strong>taining<br />

single words. Similar findings were reported by Ehri (1977) and<br />

Underwood et al. (1984). Rosinski (1977) showed that <strong>the</strong> se-<br />

mantic gradient for words that differentially relate to colors was<br />

THE STROOP EFFECT 185<br />

also c<strong>on</strong>sistent from Grade 2 through college. Merrill et al.<br />

(1981) went <strong>on</strong> to show that, with sentence c<strong>on</strong>texts, good com-<br />

prehenders in Grade 5 displayed interference <strong>on</strong>ly for targets<br />

appropriate to <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>text, whereas poor comprehenders<br />

showed interference regardless <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>text appropriateness. A<br />

study by Kareev (I 980) fur<strong>the</strong>r investigated development <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> sen-<br />

tence encoding in children using <strong>the</strong> color-word task.<br />

Interestingly, children with reading disabilities <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>ten show<br />

robust <strong>Stroop</strong> interference (Alwitt, 1966), as do autistic children<br />

(Brys<strong>on</strong>, 1983), hyperactive children (DeHaas & Young, 1984),<br />

aphasics (Cohen, Meier, & SchuLze, 1983), and retardates (Bas-<br />

sett & Schellman, 1976; Tschopp & Jorswieck, 1976). Recent<br />

work by Ellis, Woodley-Zanthos, Dulaney, and Palmer (1989)<br />

suggests that automatic word reading may be even harder for<br />

retardates to c<strong>on</strong>trol than it is for normals, causing greater inter-<br />

ference in retardates. Das (1970) reported a similar result, al-<br />

though <strong>the</strong> pattern reversed when very poor readers were in-<br />

cluded in <strong>the</strong> retarded group (Das, 1969).<br />

The overall picture <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> development, <strong>the</strong>n, can be summa-<br />

rized fairly c<strong>on</strong>cisely by <strong>the</strong> Comalli et al. (1962) result. Here is<br />

<strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong>: Interference begins early in <strong>the</strong> school years, ris-<br />

ing to its highest level around Grades 2 to 3 as reading skill de-<br />

velops. With c<strong>on</strong>tinued development <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reading, interference de-<br />

clines through <strong>the</strong> adult years until approximately age 60, at<br />

which point it begins to increase again. Virtually every<strong>on</strong>e who<br />

can read shows a robust <strong>Stroop</strong> effect from an early age.<br />

Hemispheric Differences<br />

A number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> investigators have examined laterality in <strong>the</strong><br />

<strong>Stroop</strong> task, using Dyer's (1973a) study as a springboard. Basi-<br />

calls he showed that presenting <strong>the</strong> word and <strong>the</strong> color sepa-<br />

rately to <strong>the</strong> left and right <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> fixati<strong>on</strong> did not destroy <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong><br />

effect: Both interference and facilitati<strong>on</strong> were still evident. Fur-<br />

<strong>the</strong>rmore, <strong>the</strong> effect does not seem to be altered by presenting<br />

<strong>the</strong> words vertically (McCown & Arnoult, 1981), although it<br />

may be somewhat reduced in <strong>the</strong> periphery as opposed to fo-<br />

veally (Goolkasian, 1978). Such findings opened <strong>the</strong> door for<br />

hemispheric-differences research.<br />

Schmit and Davis (197z~) tested subjects with <strong>Stroop</strong> stimuli<br />

displayed in <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>the</strong> left or right visual field using a butt<strong>on</strong>press<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se. They observed greater interference in <strong>the</strong> left hemi-<br />

sphere, c<strong>on</strong>sistent with that hemisphere's dominant role in ver-<br />

bal processing. Guiard (1981) corroborated this using a man-<br />

ual-resp<strong>on</strong>se task. The finding was also c<strong>on</strong>firmed in <strong>the</strong> stan-<br />

dard oral-resp<strong>on</strong>se procedure by Tsao, Feustel, and Soseos<br />

(1979), although <strong>on</strong>ly in <strong>the</strong>ir error data (see also Newman,<br />

1981). However, not all investigators have agreed: Warren and<br />

Marsh (1978) found equivalent interference for <strong>the</strong> two hemi-<br />

spheres using color-word stimuli, whereas L<strong>on</strong>g and Lyman<br />

(1987) found greater interference when <strong>the</strong> word was processed<br />

in <strong>the</strong> right hemisphere (with <strong>the</strong> color presented foveally).<br />

Tsao, Wu, and Feustel (1981) showed that <strong>the</strong> Schmit and<br />

Davis pattern was reversed for Chinese speakers, c<strong>on</strong>sistent<br />

with <strong>the</strong> idea that both logographs and colors are handled by<br />

<strong>the</strong> right hemisphere. <strong>An</strong>alogously, Morikawa (1981) showed<br />

that idiographic Kanji characters produced greater right-hemi-<br />

sphere interference in Japanese subjects. Hatta (1981; see also<br />

Hatta, Katoh, & Aitani, 1983) c<strong>on</strong>firmed <strong>the</strong> result for Kanji,


186 COLIN M. MACLEOD<br />

but found no hemispheric difference for orthographic Kana.<br />

Interestingly, Morikawa emphasized <strong>the</strong> overall reduced size <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

interference effects with Japanese subjects, itself a result worth<br />

pursuing.<br />

Cohen and Martin (1975) presented auditory stimuli to <strong>the</strong><br />

left or right ear using <strong>the</strong> Hamers (1973) high/low procedure.<br />

Again, <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> effect was larger in <strong>the</strong> left hemisphere, and<br />

was accentuated by simultaneous irrelevant distracti<strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong><br />

o<strong>the</strong>r ear (see also Vaid & Lambert, 1979, with <strong>the</strong> same task,<br />

and Pieters, 1981, using an auditory left/right positi<strong>on</strong> versus<br />

word-discriminati<strong>on</strong> task). Alivisatos and Wilding (1982) used<br />

<strong>the</strong> global/local-letters task (with a manual resp<strong>on</strong>se) to show<br />

that <strong>the</strong> local level <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a foveally presented first stimulus inter-<br />

fered with processing a sec<strong>on</strong>d stimulus in <strong>the</strong> right hemisphere<br />

but not in <strong>the</strong> left. The global aspect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> first stimulus inter-<br />

fered with <strong>the</strong> sec<strong>on</strong>d stimulus in both hemispheres, with a<br />

tendency to more interference in <strong>the</strong> right hemisphere.<br />

Toma and Tsao (1985) obtained greater interference in <strong>the</strong><br />

left hemisphere for <strong>the</strong> picture-word task. Using <strong>the</strong> same task,<br />

Lupker and Sanders (1982) hypo<strong>the</strong>sized that <strong>the</strong> verbal-re-<br />

sp<strong>on</strong>se competiti<strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong> left hemisphere gave way to a percep-<br />

tual type <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interference in <strong>the</strong> right hemisphere, a hypo<strong>the</strong>sis<br />

worthy <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> fur<strong>the</strong>r investigati<strong>on</strong>. In particular, though, more<br />

work is needed where <strong>the</strong> two dimensi<strong>on</strong>s are presented sepa-<br />

rately to <strong>the</strong> two hemispheres ra<strong>the</strong>r than toge<strong>the</strong>r to <strong>the</strong> same<br />

hemisphere. SOA studies might also be informative by c<strong>on</strong>trol-<br />

ling which hemisphere is stimulated first and by which dimen-<br />

si<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> stimulus.<br />

Evoked (event-related) potentials are part <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a new technology<br />

for studying <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> effect. Scott, H<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>fman, and Bickford<br />

(1967) were pi<strong>on</strong>eers in this area, dem<strong>on</strong>strating that lambda<br />

waves in <strong>the</strong> parietal-occipital regi<strong>on</strong> showed increased ampli-<br />

tude in <strong>the</strong> interference c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, although <strong>the</strong>y admitted that<br />

this Could relate to muscle potentials as easily as to mental activ-<br />

ity. The technology has advanced a great deal since <strong>the</strong>n (e.g.,<br />

Johnst<strong>on</strong> & Venables, 1982). More in <strong>the</strong> current mold <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> such<br />

studies, Warren and Marsh (1979) took <strong>the</strong>ir neural-activity<br />

measurements to suggest that standard <strong>Stroop</strong> interference was<br />

due to resp<strong>on</strong>se-selecti<strong>on</strong> processes, but that <strong>the</strong> reverse <strong>Stroop</strong><br />

effect was due to complex encoding interacti<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Duncan-Johns<strong>on</strong> and KopeU 0980,1981) used oral resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

time toge<strong>the</strong>r with P300 latency (cf. Pritchard, 1981) in <strong>the</strong><br />

discrete-trials versi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> task with c<strong>on</strong>gruent, inc<strong>on</strong>-<br />

gruent, and neutral words and two ink colors. Blocks <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> trials<br />

required word reading or ink-color naming. The usual pattern<br />

was observed in <strong>the</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>se times, but P300 was unaffected by<br />

experimental c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. Because P300 is taken to be an index<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> encoding, <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong> was that interference must stem<br />

from resp<strong>on</strong>se competiti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Aine and Harter (1984a, 1984b) adapted <strong>the</strong> study <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> evoked<br />

potentials to examining hemispheric differences in <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong><br />

task. Basically, <strong>the</strong>y claimed that <strong>the</strong> activity associated with<br />

interference occurs in <strong>the</strong> left hemisphere, c<strong>on</strong>verging <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se time and error-rate measures already described. Tak-<br />

ing all <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> this evidence toge<strong>the</strong>r, <strong>the</strong> following c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong> is<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>fered: The left hemisphere generally shows more interference<br />

than <strong>the</strong> right. A study by Posner, Walker, Friedrich, and Rafal<br />

(1984) suggests that <strong>the</strong> left parietal lobe may be particularly<br />

involved in disengaging attenti<strong>on</strong>, although <strong>the</strong> left fr<strong>on</strong>tal lobe<br />

may also play a role (Perret, 1974). Undoubtedly, we will see<br />

more work <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> topic <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> localizati<strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong> near future.<br />

Language Differences: The Case <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> Bilingual<br />

If <strong>Stroop</strong> interference stems from an irresistible urge to read<br />

<strong>the</strong> word, what would happen if you were naming ink colors in<br />

<strong>on</strong>e language but <strong>the</strong> words were in ano<strong>the</strong>r language? About<br />

two dozen studies have been directed at this questi<strong>on</strong>. This<br />

work began with Dalrymple-Alford (1968), but his items were<br />

ei<strong>the</strong>r inc<strong>on</strong>gruent items in <strong>the</strong> same language or c<strong>on</strong>gruent<br />

items in different languages, a c<strong>on</strong>found that does not permit us<br />

to obtain a clear picture <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> effect within versus be-<br />

tween languages. For this, we must examine studies by Prest<strong>on</strong><br />

and Lambert (1969) and by Dyer (197 la).<br />

Prest<strong>on</strong> and Lambert (1969) found substantial interference<br />

whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> ink colors were to be named in <strong>the</strong> same language<br />

as <strong>the</strong> distracting words appeared or in <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r language. En-<br />

glish-Hungarian bilinguals showed roughly 68% <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> interfer-<br />

ence across languages that <strong>the</strong>y showed within a single lan-<br />

guage; for English-French bilinguals, <strong>the</strong> overlap was 95%. Pres-<br />

t<strong>on</strong> and Lambert c<strong>on</strong>cluded that interference between<br />

languages could be as great as that within languages, but that it<br />

depended <strong>on</strong> relative familiarity with <strong>the</strong> two languages in ques-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Dyer (1971 a) showed that English m<strong>on</strong>olinguals naming col-<br />

ors in English displayed maximal interference when <strong>the</strong> words<br />

were in English and declining interference as <strong>the</strong> similarity <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

<strong>the</strong> irrelevant words to English decreased. Using Spanish-Eng-<br />

lish bilinguals in his sec<strong>on</strong>d experiment, Dyer found maximal<br />

interference when <strong>the</strong> naming and distracting languages coin-<br />

cided, but <strong>the</strong>re was also substantial interference when <strong>the</strong> lan-<br />

guages did not coincide. Roughly, <strong>the</strong> different-language c<strong>on</strong>di-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong>s showed about 63% <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> interference seen in <strong>the</strong> same-<br />

language c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Shortly after Dyer's study, Hamers (1973; Hamers & Lam-<br />

bert, 1972) used her auditory <strong>Stroop</strong> analog with French-Eng-<br />

lish bilinguals and reached <strong>the</strong> same c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong>: The different-<br />

language c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> produced about 76% <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> interference<br />

seen in <strong>the</strong> same-language c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. This pattern recurs for<br />

Spanish-English bilinguals in <strong>the</strong> picture-word task (Ehri &<br />

Ryan, 1980), French-English bilinguals in <strong>the</strong> flanker-word task<br />

(Guttentag et al., 1984), and diverse languages in <strong>the</strong> color-word<br />

task (e.g., Chinese-English and Japanese-English; Fang, Tzeng,<br />

& Alva, 1981; Turkish-English: Kiyak, 1982). Fang et al. (1981)<br />

also c<strong>on</strong>firmed that <strong>the</strong> ratio <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> between-language to within-<br />

language interference declines as language similarity decreases.<br />

Complicating matters, <strong>the</strong> weightings <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interference within<br />

and between languages are also affected by relative pr<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>iciency<br />

in <strong>the</strong> two languages. M~igiste (1984,1985) observed a changing<br />

pattern with <strong>the</strong> development <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> individual's sec<strong>on</strong>d lan-<br />

guage. Initially, her subjects were German dominant and<br />

showed more interference when resp<strong>on</strong>ding in German regard-<br />

less <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> words were Swedish or German. Gradually,<br />

as <strong>the</strong>ir experience with Swedish increased, <strong>the</strong>y reached a<br />

point <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> equivalence; <strong>the</strong>n, as Swedish came to dominate, <strong>the</strong>y<br />

showed most interference when naming in Swedish. Chen and<br />

Ho (1986), using Chinese-English bilinguals, suggested that<br />

within-language interference was always greater for <strong>the</strong>ir Chi-


nese-dominant subjects, but that <strong>the</strong> pattern when naming in<br />

English shifts with increased English-language experience.<br />

From initially greater between-language interference, <strong>the</strong>ir sub-<br />

jects moved to finally greater within-language interference.<br />

There is also evidence to suggest that this difference in interfer-<br />

ence depends <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> dominance <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> naming language. If <strong>the</strong><br />

naming language is <strong>the</strong> n<strong>on</strong>dominant <strong>on</strong>e, interference be-<br />

tween and within languages tends to be close to identical (e.g.,<br />

Dornic, 1982; Dornic & Wirberg, 1983).<br />

Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, certain languages, such as Chinese, may be<br />

more vulnerable to interference than o<strong>the</strong>rs. Biederman and<br />

Tsao (1979; see also Morikawa, 1981, using Japanese subjects)<br />

showed much greater interference for Chinese students naming<br />

<strong>the</strong> ink colors <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> logographic characters than for English stu-<br />

dents naming <strong>the</strong> ink colors <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words. They c<strong>on</strong>cluded that <strong>the</strong><br />

processes involved in reading Chinese are more similar to those<br />

involved in naming colors than are those involved in reading<br />

English. Tsao et al. (1981; see also Morikawa, 1981, using Japa-<br />

nese subjects) provided some support for this claim by showing<br />

that more interference occurred in <strong>the</strong> right hemisphere for<br />

Chinese speakers, unlike <strong>the</strong> usual pattern <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> maximal interfer-<br />

ence in <strong>the</strong> left hemisphere for speakers <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> romance languages.<br />

However, Smith and Kirsner (1982) obtained more symmetri-<br />

cal data using Chinese-English and French-English bilinguals,<br />

so this issue has yet to be resolved.<br />

What can we c<strong>on</strong>clude about <strong>Stroop</strong> interference in bilin-<br />

guals for <strong>the</strong> present? Interference between <strong>the</strong> two languages <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a<br />

bilingual, although not as great as that within ei<strong>the</strong>r <strong>on</strong>e <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong><br />

languages, is very robust: Between-language interference typi-<br />

cally is about 75% <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> within-language interference. Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore,<br />

a dominant language has more potential for interfering than does<br />

a n<strong>on</strong>dominant <strong>on</strong>e. There may also be differences in <strong>the</strong> pro-<br />

cessing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> orthographic and idiographic languages. Overall, <strong>the</strong><br />

cross-language semantic c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong> to <strong>Stroop</strong> interference is<br />

substantial.<br />

Theoretical Accounts <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> <strong>Effect</strong><br />

It seems <strong>on</strong>ly appropriate to preface discussi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> current<br />

<strong>the</strong>ories with a comment <strong>on</strong> how <strong>Stroop</strong> himself interpreted his<br />

data. Here is what he c<strong>on</strong>cluded over a half-century ago:<br />

The associati<strong>on</strong>s that have been formed between <strong>the</strong> word stimuli<br />

and <strong>the</strong> reading resp<strong>on</strong>se are evidently more effective than those<br />

that have been formed between <strong>the</strong> color stimuli and <strong>the</strong> naming<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se. Since <strong>the</strong>se associati<strong>on</strong>s are products <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> training, and<br />

since <strong>the</strong> difference in <strong>the</strong>ir strength corresp<strong>on</strong>ds roughly to <strong>the</strong><br />

difference in training in reading words and naming colors, it<br />

seems reas<strong>on</strong>able to c<strong>on</strong>clude that <strong>the</strong> difference in speed in read-<br />

ing names <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> colors and in naming colors may be satisfactorily<br />

accounted for by <strong>the</strong> difference in training in <strong>the</strong> two activities.<br />

(<strong>Stroop</strong>, 1935b, pp. 659-660)<br />

<strong>Stroop</strong>'s (1935b, 1938) general view corresp<strong>on</strong>ded closely to<br />

<strong>on</strong>e <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> two prevalent accounts <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> effecth<strong>the</strong><br />

relative speed-<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>-processing view--and was compatible with<br />

<strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r <strong>on</strong>e--<strong>the</strong> automaticity view. Because <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>ir preemi-<br />

nence in <strong>the</strong> literature, and because <strong>the</strong>y are c<strong>on</strong>ceptually close<br />

cousins, <strong>the</strong>se two explanati<strong>on</strong>s are examined first.<br />

Relative Speed <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Processing<br />

In its simplest form, <strong>the</strong> relative speed-<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>-processing view<br />

begins with <strong>the</strong> fact that words are read faster than colors are<br />

THE STROOP EFFECT 187<br />

named (e.g., Cattell, 1886; Fraisse, 1969). This speed difference<br />

is seen as particularly critical when two potential resp<strong>on</strong>ses (e.g.,<br />

<strong>on</strong>e from a word and <strong>on</strong>e from an ink color) compete to be <strong>the</strong><br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se actually produced. The time cost <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> this competiti<strong>on</strong><br />

is "interference." This general interpretati<strong>on</strong> is referred to as<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se competiti<strong>on</strong> occurring at <strong>the</strong> end <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a horse race, be-<br />

cause <strong>the</strong> two codes are seen as racing to c<strong>on</strong>trol final output.<br />

A quarter <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a century ago, Klein (1964) saw interference as<br />

resulting from <strong>the</strong> need to "restimulate" with <strong>the</strong> ink color to<br />

overcome <strong>the</strong> str<strong>on</strong>g tendency to produce <strong>the</strong> word, although<br />

<strong>the</strong> mechanism <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>restimulati<strong>on</strong> was not specified. Klein's view<br />

was not altoge<strong>the</strong>r inc<strong>on</strong>sistent with <strong>Stroop</strong>'s, and it character-<br />

ized earlier attempts to explain <strong>the</strong> SWoop effect in terms <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

"habit strength" and related associati<strong>on</strong>istic ideas <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> learning.<br />

Today most psychologists think <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> task as a hallmark<br />

measure <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> attenti<strong>on</strong>, not learning. Thus, Treisman (1969) saw<br />

<strong>the</strong> problem as difficulty in deciding whe<strong>the</strong>r to attend to <strong>the</strong><br />

ink color or to <strong>the</strong> word analyzers when <strong>the</strong>y led to different<br />

potential resp<strong>on</strong>ses, an idea Dyer (1973c) adopted in his review.<br />

Ideas like Treisman's underlie what came to be <strong>the</strong> best<br />

known realizati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> resp<strong>on</strong>se competiti<strong>on</strong>: those <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Mort<strong>on</strong><br />

(1969; Mort<strong>on</strong> & Chambers, 1973) and Posner and Snyder<br />

(1975). It was this general resp<strong>on</strong>se-competiti<strong>on</strong> idea that Dyer<br />

(1973c) endorsed in his review. In <strong>the</strong> words <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Mort<strong>on</strong> and<br />

Chambers (1973),<br />

The various stimulus analyzers [for word and ink color] are sup-<br />

posed to operate in parallel and will each give rise to an appro-<br />

priate naming resp<strong>on</strong>se. If multiple resp<strong>on</strong>ses are available <strong>the</strong>y<br />

will compete for entry into <strong>the</strong> single channel exit from <strong>the</strong> Logo-<br />

gen System... <strong>the</strong> crucial variable is <strong>the</strong> relative speed <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> naming<br />

<strong>the</strong> various attributes <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> stimuli. (p. 388)<br />

This is a very clear statement <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> relative speed-<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>-processing<br />

view, including its resp<strong>on</strong>se-competiti<strong>on</strong> element.<br />

Very similar ideas are c<strong>on</strong>tained in <strong>the</strong> account <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>fered by<br />

Posner and Snyder (t975):<br />

First, <strong>the</strong> usual <strong>Stroop</strong> effect arises because <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> resp<strong>on</strong>se competi-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong> between vocal resp<strong>on</strong>ses to <strong>the</strong> printed word and <strong>the</strong> ink<br />

color.... Sec<strong>on</strong>d, <strong>the</strong> directi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interference depends up<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

time relati<strong>on</strong>s involved. Words are read faster than colors can be<br />

named, thus a color naming resp<strong>on</strong>se receives str<strong>on</strong>ger interfer-<br />

ence from <strong>the</strong> word than <strong>the</strong> reverse .... Third, words <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>ten facili-<br />

tate <strong>the</strong> vocal output to colors with which <strong>the</strong>y share a comm<strong>on</strong><br />

name .... These three results suggest that color naming and read-<br />

ing go <strong>on</strong> in parallel and without interference until close to <strong>the</strong><br />

output. (p. 57)<br />

Again, <strong>the</strong> elements <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> relative speed and interference at <strong>the</strong><br />

stage <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> resp<strong>on</strong>se output are highlighted. These are <strong>the</strong> two<br />

essential elements <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> relative speed-<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>-processing account<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> effect. Appendix B summarizes <strong>the</strong> 18 major re-<br />

sults that must be handled by any successful <strong>the</strong>ory. Let us<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sider now how this evidence accords with this account.<br />

Certainly, <strong>the</strong> relative speed-<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>-processing idea allows for nu-<br />

merous analogs <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> task (Result 1); all that is required is<br />

differential processing time for two competing stimuli. Regard-<br />

ing <strong>the</strong> effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> n<strong>on</strong>semantic relati<strong>on</strong>s (Result 2), this view<br />

holds that greater similarity between <strong>the</strong> names <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> two compet-<br />

ing resp<strong>on</strong>ses should make it harder to select <strong>the</strong> correct <strong>on</strong>e at<br />

<strong>the</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>se-producti<strong>on</strong> stage. Regarding semantic effects (Re-


188 COLIN M. MACLEOD<br />

suit 3), disambiguati<strong>on</strong> at <strong>the</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>se stage should also be<br />

more difficult for more highly related resp<strong>on</strong>ses.<br />

Posner and Snyder (1975) pointed out that words more highly<br />

related to <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>cept <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> color would be primed by processing<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> ink-color names, increasing <strong>the</strong> likelihood <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> those words<br />

reaching <strong>the</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>se stage first. This logic explains <strong>the</strong> fact<br />

that overlap <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> resp<strong>on</strong>se set for <strong>the</strong> relevant and irrelevant di-<br />

mensi<strong>on</strong>s influences interference (Result 14); <strong>the</strong> two dimen-<br />

si<strong>on</strong>s are priming each o<strong>the</strong>r. Priming also explains why n<strong>on</strong>-<br />

color words preceded by related words cause more interference<br />

in color naming (Result 4). Basically, words are processed faster<br />

when primed and hence are more likely to win <strong>the</strong> race to <strong>the</strong><br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se stage and to interfere with color naming. Of course,<br />

<strong>the</strong> priming mechanism is not fundamental to <strong>the</strong> relative-<br />

speed hypo<strong>the</strong>sis per se. It must be grafted <strong>on</strong>, and represents an<br />

additi<strong>on</strong>al element to this explanati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Facilitatory c<strong>on</strong>gruence effects (Result 5) should occur be-<br />

cause <strong>the</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>se to both dimensi<strong>on</strong>s is <strong>the</strong> same, so produc-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong> can be based <strong>on</strong> whichever arrives first at <strong>the</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

stage. Indeed, <strong>the</strong>y probably even prime each o<strong>the</strong>r. Increased<br />

interference from mixing c<strong>on</strong>gruent with inc<strong>on</strong>gruent trials<br />

(Result 7) might be thought <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> as being due to greater uncer-<br />

tainty at <strong>the</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>se stage regarding which dimensi<strong>on</strong> (<strong>on</strong> in-<br />

c<strong>on</strong>gruent trials) is to provide <strong>the</strong> output. Of course, this re-<br />

quires an additi<strong>on</strong>al assumpti<strong>on</strong> bey<strong>on</strong>d <strong>the</strong> two main <strong>on</strong>es<br />

menti<strong>on</strong>ed earlier, but it seems a reas<strong>on</strong>able <strong>on</strong>e.<br />

Although <strong>the</strong> fact that integrated stimuli result in more inter-<br />

ference than n<strong>on</strong>integrated stimuli (Result 6) is not a direct<br />

predicti<strong>on</strong> from <strong>the</strong> relative speed-<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>-processing view, it can be<br />

handled. Assume that it is easier to ascertain which potential<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se derived from which stimulus dimensi<strong>on</strong> when <strong>the</strong> di-<br />

mensi<strong>on</strong>s are more easily separated. Dealing with <strong>the</strong> fact that<br />

manual resp<strong>on</strong>ding displays less interference than oral re-<br />

sp<strong>on</strong>ding (Result 13) is even more straightforward. Simply as-<br />

sume that <strong>the</strong>re are different resp<strong>on</strong>se-output routines (or<br />

buffers) for different modalities, which also fits with effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

stimulus-resp<strong>on</strong>se compatibility.<br />

Pretrial cues should assist processing (Result 9) because <strong>the</strong>y<br />

can be used to "tune" <strong>the</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>se-output system. These cues<br />

help in deciding which dimensi<strong>on</strong> is salient and perhaps also<br />

what value to expect <strong>on</strong> that dimensi<strong>on</strong>. However, too many<br />

so-called cues would probably lead <strong>the</strong> subject to ignore <strong>the</strong>m,<br />

as has been found. What about sequential trial effects (Result<br />

8)? Two problems arise in <strong>the</strong> system. First, <strong>the</strong>re may be some<br />

trace <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> two potential resp<strong>on</strong>ses from <strong>the</strong> previous trial still<br />

resident in <strong>the</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>se buffer when <strong>the</strong> subsequent trial be-<br />

gins. Sec<strong>on</strong>d, <strong>the</strong>re is probably priming <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>ses <strong>on</strong> trial<br />

n by <strong>the</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>ses <strong>on</strong> trial n - 1. With some jockeying, this<br />

horse race can be made sensitive to neighboring trials.<br />

Although silent <strong>on</strong> sex differences (Result 15), <strong>the</strong> relative<br />

speed view would predict greater left hemisphere involvement<br />

(Result 17) because <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> importance <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reading. Both <strong>the</strong> de-<br />

velopmental pattern (Result 16) and <strong>the</strong> bilingual pattern (Re-<br />

sult 18) make sense under this view because both development<br />

and language pr<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>iciency ought to affect word processing<br />

speed.<br />

So far, so good. Fifteen <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> 18 results are comfortably ac-<br />

commodated. However, <strong>the</strong> remaining three--without doubt<br />

<strong>the</strong> most direct predicti<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> relative speed-<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>-processing<br />

view--appear to be wr<strong>on</strong>g. The most obvious predicti<strong>on</strong> is that<br />

<strong>the</strong> relative speed <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing each <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> two dimensi<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

<strong>the</strong> compound stimulus should determine interference. The<br />

naive intuiti<strong>on</strong> is that <strong>the</strong> faster dimensi<strong>on</strong> should interfere with<br />

<strong>the</strong> slower <strong>on</strong>e and not vice versa; interference should always be<br />

completely asymmetrical. Of course, this extreme positi<strong>on</strong> need<br />

not be true, in fact, because we are dealing with distributi<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

processing times over trials, and any overlap in those distribu-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong>s could produce interference. Figure I portrays this point; it<br />

is apparent that orderly patterns <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interference are predicted.<br />

In fact, though, interference is not an orderly functi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

relative speed <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing: The careful SOA studies <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Glaser<br />

and Glaser (1982) and Glaser and Dttngelh<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>f (1984) c<strong>on</strong>vinc-<br />

ingly make this point (see SOA). Previewing <strong>the</strong> slower dimen-<br />

si<strong>on</strong> does not lead to <strong>the</strong> clear reversal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> directi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> inter-<br />

ference expected, <strong>the</strong>reby violating two major results (Results<br />

10 and 11). Also relevant are <strong>the</strong> studies c<strong>on</strong>ducted by Kevin<br />

Dunbar and myself (Dunbar & MacLeod, 1984; MacLeod &<br />

Dunbar, 1988): Direct manipulati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> speed <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a dimensi<strong>on</strong> through reorientati<strong>on</strong> or practice (Result 12)<br />

does not produce results c<strong>on</strong>sistent with this <strong>the</strong>oretical per-<br />

spective.<br />

The relative speed-<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>-processing hypo<strong>the</strong>sis c<strong>on</strong>tains three<br />

key assumpti<strong>on</strong>s. First, <strong>the</strong>re is assumed to be parallel process-<br />

ing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> two dimensi<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> stimulus at differential speeds.<br />

Sec<strong>on</strong>d, <strong>the</strong>re is a limited-capacity resp<strong>on</strong>se channel into which<br />

<strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> two potential resp<strong>on</strong>ses can be admitted at a<br />

time; priority is determined by speed. Third, <strong>the</strong>re is potential<br />

for priming <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> possible resp<strong>on</strong>ses from several sources, includ-<br />

ing preceding trials and o<strong>the</strong>r resp<strong>on</strong>se-set elements. This is<br />

actually quite a powerful set <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> assumpti<strong>on</strong>s. Toge<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong>y are<br />

capable <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> accommodating 15 <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> 18 critical findings in Ap-<br />

pendix B, although some corollary assumpti<strong>on</strong>s were intro-<br />

duced al<strong>on</strong>g <strong>the</strong> way. However, <strong>the</strong>y fail in <strong>the</strong> face <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> most<br />

direct test <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> view: explicit manipulati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> relative speed<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing by SOA or by practice. On this basis, I c<strong>on</strong>tend<br />

that <strong>the</strong> relative speed-<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>-processing hypo<strong>the</strong>sis must be re-<br />

jected.<br />

Automaticity<br />

The sec<strong>on</strong>d prevalent explanati<strong>on</strong> is <strong>the</strong> automaticity ac-<br />

count, which was rooted in Cattell's (1886) work over a century<br />

ago. Here <strong>the</strong> basic idea is that processing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong>e dimensi<strong>on</strong><br />

requires much more attenti<strong>on</strong> than does processing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r<br />

dimensi<strong>on</strong>. Thus, naming <strong>the</strong> ink color draws more heavily <strong>on</strong><br />

attenti<strong>on</strong>al resources than does reading <strong>the</strong> irrelevant word.<br />

Moreover, reading <strong>the</strong> word is seen as obligatory, whereas nam-<br />

ing <strong>the</strong> ink color is not. Presumably, this imbalance derives<br />

from our extensive history <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reading words as opposed to nam-<br />

ing ink colors. Under this view, <strong>the</strong> asymmetry that is <strong>the</strong> fun-<br />

damental characteristic <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> task must occur. Words<br />

are read very automatically; colors require c<strong>on</strong>siderably more<br />

attenti<strong>on</strong> to be named.<br />

This descripti<strong>on</strong> is based <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>the</strong>orizing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> LaBerge and<br />

Samuels (1974), Posner and Snyder (1975), Shiffrin and<br />

Schneider (1977), and Logan (1978) am<strong>on</strong>g o<strong>the</strong>rs (see, e.g.,<br />

Hunt & Lansman, 1986). C<strong>on</strong>trary to <strong>the</strong> all-or-n<strong>on</strong>e view <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>


THE STROOP EFFECT 189<br />

Figure 1. Relative speed <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing and interference. As <strong>the</strong> latency distributi<strong>on</strong>s for <strong>the</strong> processing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

<strong>the</strong> two dimensi<strong>on</strong>s shift, so should <strong>the</strong> patterns <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interference.<br />

automaticity popular in sec<strong>on</strong>dary sources, all <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>se investi-<br />

gators saw automaticity as a gradient that developed with learn-<br />

ing. Thus, word reading was very automatic, and color (or pic-<br />

ture) naming was much less automatic. More automatic process-<br />

ing could <strong>the</strong>n interfere with less automatic processing, but not<br />

vice versa. The <strong>Stroop</strong> task is an interesting case expressly be-<br />

cause <strong>the</strong> two dimensi<strong>on</strong>s differ so much in how automatically<br />

<strong>the</strong>y are processed.<br />

Let us c<strong>on</strong>sider how such a view comes to grips with <strong>the</strong> 18<br />

key results in Appendix B. Certainly whenever <strong>the</strong> irrelevant<br />

dimensi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a two-dimensi<strong>on</strong>al stimulus is a word, <strong>the</strong> poten-<br />

tial for interference exists. Thus, many analogs <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong><br />

color-word task (Result 1) are expected. Although an all-or-<br />

n<strong>on</strong>e versi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>automaticity would have trouble handling inter-<br />

ference in situati<strong>on</strong>s where nei<strong>the</strong>r dimensi<strong>on</strong> was a word, this<br />

would not be <strong>the</strong> case for <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tinuum versi<strong>on</strong> (e.g., MacLeod<br />

& Dunbar, 1988).<br />

The real strength <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> automaticity <strong>the</strong>ories lies in <strong>the</strong>ir ability<br />

to capture priming situati<strong>on</strong>s. Recall that priming required an<br />

additi<strong>on</strong>al assumpti<strong>on</strong> for <strong>the</strong> speed-<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>-processing account. In<br />

c<strong>on</strong>trast, it is at <strong>the</strong> very heart <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> automaticity explanati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

as is evident in reading Posner and Snyder (1975), for example.<br />

Priming occurs through <strong>the</strong> automatic spread <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> activati<strong>on</strong>, es-<br />

sentially <strong>the</strong> same mechanism used to explain interference.<br />

Thus, orthographic/acoustic effects (Result 2) should result<br />

from automatic activati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> parts <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> relevant and irrelevant<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se words. In like manner, semantic effects should be evi-<br />

dent, particularly given that Posner and Snyder aimed to ex-<br />

plain lexical facilitati<strong>on</strong> with <strong>the</strong> same mechanism as SWoop<br />

interference (see also Logan, 1980, for a similar idea). As <strong>the</strong>


190 COLIN M. MACLEOD<br />

irrelevant words become more color related, <strong>the</strong>y automatically<br />

activate more comm<strong>on</strong> features with <strong>the</strong> relevant ink colors,<br />

resulting in interference (Result 3). Primes should be able to<br />

accomplish this in a prime-target procedure (Result 4), and<br />

previous trials should be able to do it in a c<strong>on</strong>tinuous-trials<br />

procedure (Result 8). Set overlap should also influence <strong>the</strong> ex-<br />

tent <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> automatic activati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> competing features <strong>on</strong> any given<br />

trial through priming (Result 14). Finally, facilitati<strong>on</strong> from c<strong>on</strong>-<br />

gruent dimensi<strong>on</strong>s ought to occur (Result 5). In sum, automatic<br />

priming is <strong>the</strong> most "natural" c<strong>on</strong>sequence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> automaticity<br />

account.<br />

Also a straightforward outgrowth <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> automaticity ac-<br />

count is <strong>the</strong> fact that practice affects interference (Results 12).<br />

Practice can also be seen as encompassing <strong>the</strong> changes that<br />

occur with <strong>the</strong> development <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reading skill (Result 16) and with<br />

increasing language pr<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>iciency in bilinguals (Result 18). Auto-<br />

matic processes (e.g., reading words) have to be learned; <strong>the</strong><br />

more automatic a process becomes with practice, <strong>the</strong> more it is<br />

capable <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> causing interference with a less automatic process.<br />

This also helps to explain apparent instances <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reversed <strong>Stroop</strong><br />

interference (Result I l): When a normally more automatic pro-<br />

cess associated with <strong>on</strong>e stimulus dimensi<strong>on</strong> is altered by an<br />

experimental manipulati<strong>on</strong>, <strong>the</strong> normally less automatic pro-<br />

cess associated with <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r dimensi<strong>on</strong> may become relatively<br />

more automatic. In such an instance, <strong>the</strong> directi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interfer-<br />

ence reverses.<br />

The modality effect--that interference is reduced when <strong>the</strong><br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se mode is switched from oral to manual (Result 13)-<br />

makes sense because, although it may be quite automatic to<br />

process a word for oral output, it may be less automatic when<br />

<strong>the</strong> required resp<strong>on</strong>se is in some o<strong>the</strong>r modality. Of course, this<br />

is an additi<strong>on</strong>al assumpti<strong>on</strong>, but not an unreas<strong>on</strong>able <strong>on</strong>e.<br />

The effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> SPA (Result 10) are much less nettlesome for<br />

<strong>the</strong> automaticity view, which does not hinge <strong>on</strong> processing<br />

speed. Indeed, <strong>the</strong> fact that SPA manipulati<strong>on</strong> has little impact<br />

<strong>on</strong> word reading fits with <strong>the</strong> automaticity view. Unfortunately,<br />

this advantage is <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>fset by <strong>the</strong> fact that tests <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> automaticity are<br />

less direct than tests <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> speed. Probably for this reas<strong>on</strong>, <strong>Stroop</strong><br />

researchers have not measured <strong>the</strong> automaticity <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> two di-<br />

mensi<strong>on</strong>s directly (cf. <strong>the</strong> discussi<strong>on</strong> in MacLeod & Dunbar,<br />

1988, p. 134).<br />

Like <strong>the</strong> speed <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing view, <strong>the</strong> automaticity account<br />

would predict a greater role for <strong>the</strong> left hemisphere (Result 17),<br />

because reading is what is becoming automated. There is also<br />

nothing in this account that predicts sex differences (Result 15).<br />

The remaining three results in Appendix B pose more serious<br />

threats to <strong>the</strong> automaticity account. First, as l<strong>on</strong>g as both di-<br />

mensi<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a display are perceivable simultaneously, it might<br />

Seem that whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong>y are separate or integrated should not<br />

matter very much. In ei<strong>the</strong>r event, <strong>on</strong>ce seen, a word would be<br />

processed automatically. Yet integrati<strong>on</strong> versus separati<strong>on</strong> does<br />

matter (Result 6); generally interference effects are substantially<br />

larger in <strong>the</strong> integrated versi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> task (e.g., Kahneman &<br />

Henik, 1981).<br />

Because automatic processes do not require attenti<strong>on</strong>, strate-<br />

gies should have little effect <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir executi<strong>on</strong>. Yet particular<br />

mixtures <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>gruent and inc<strong>on</strong>gruent trials alter <strong>the</strong> degree <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

interference (Result 7), as can cues about what to expect <strong>on</strong> an<br />

upcoming trial (Result 9). These straightforward attenti<strong>on</strong>al-al-<br />

locati<strong>on</strong> effects run against <strong>the</strong> grain <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> automaticity idea.<br />

Perhaps <strong>the</strong> best illustrati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> this is <strong>the</strong> study by Kahneman<br />

and Chajczyk (1983) in which <strong>the</strong>y observed diluted <strong>Stroop</strong><br />

effects when irrelevant additi<strong>on</strong>al words occurred in <strong>the</strong> display.<br />

How could reading a word, which is claimed to require no re-<br />

sources, draw resources away from color naming? Why should<br />

an extra, unrelated word draw away fur<strong>the</strong>r resources?<br />

These questi<strong>on</strong>s are not resolved by <strong>the</strong> automaticity<br />

hypo<strong>the</strong>sis.<br />

It seems, <strong>the</strong>n, that automaticity fails to provide a compre-<br />

hensive account <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> effect. However, this failure may<br />

apply <strong>on</strong>ly to <strong>the</strong> str<strong>on</strong>g, all-or-n<strong>on</strong>e view <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>automaticity. Kah-<br />

neman and Chajczyk (1983) described a process as str<strong>on</strong>gly<br />

automatic if it is unaffected by attenti<strong>on</strong>al-allocati<strong>on</strong> strategy. A<br />

process is partly automatic if it can occur largely without atten-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong>, but never<strong>the</strong>less is affected by attenti<strong>on</strong>. They suggested<br />

that <strong>the</strong> partly automatic designati<strong>on</strong> is more appropriate for<br />

<strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> situati<strong>on</strong>, leaving room for attenti<strong>on</strong>al-allocati<strong>on</strong><br />

strategies to exert some influence.<br />

MacLeod and Dunbar (1988) reemphasized <strong>the</strong> view <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>auto-<br />

maticity as a c<strong>on</strong>tinuum, not a dichotomy. This positi<strong>on</strong> harks<br />

back to earlier explanati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> effect by reinstating<br />

learning history as a critical element in interference. Better<br />

learned processes will be more automatic and will lead to such<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sequences as interference. Although resp<strong>on</strong>se-speed im-<br />

provements may also be an outgrowth <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> practice or experience,<br />

speed per se will not be <strong>the</strong> important factor. A well-learned but<br />

slow process could interfere with a less well-learned but faster<br />

process because interference arises throughout <strong>the</strong> course <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

processing, not just at some late resp<strong>on</strong>se stage.<br />

Unfortunately, <strong>Stroop</strong> experiments rarely have provided any<br />

independent measure o fautomaticity. The specificity <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> rela-<br />

tive speed-<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>-processing view allowed it to be put to stringent<br />

test (and to fail). To provide a reas<strong>on</strong>able test <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> automaticity<br />

views, we must be able to identify a priori which processes are<br />

more automatic so that c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong>s about interference asym-<br />

metries are not entirely ad hoe. Studies carefully c<strong>on</strong>trolling<br />

learning history and using methodologies such as <strong>the</strong> sec<strong>on</strong>d-<br />

ary-task procedure hold promise in this regard. At present, auto-<br />

maticity accounts remain potentially viable, but in need <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

greater specificati<strong>on</strong> and more stringent test.<br />

Perceptual Encoding<br />

As Dyer (1973c, p. 114) noted, "Most <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>se explanati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

have c<strong>on</strong>sidered <strong>the</strong> phenomen<strong>on</strong> in terms <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> resp<strong>on</strong>se compe-<br />

titi<strong>on</strong>" Such views are <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>ten referred to as "late selecti<strong>on</strong>" ac-<br />

counts, in that <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>flict occurs late in processing at a re-<br />

sp<strong>on</strong>se stage as opposed to "early selecti<strong>on</strong>" at encoding, for<br />

example. Preference for late-selecti<strong>on</strong> accounts has been c<strong>on</strong>sis-<br />

tent throughout <strong>the</strong> history <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>Stroop</strong> research; relative speed<br />

and automaticity are just <strong>the</strong> two most prevalent examples.<br />

However, it is worthwhile to c<strong>on</strong>sider <strong>the</strong> early selecti<strong>on</strong> idea.<br />

The best known versi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> early selecti<strong>on</strong> was put forward by<br />

Hock and Egeth (1970): <strong>the</strong> perceptual-encoding account. The<br />

basic idea is that perceptual encoding <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> ink-color informati<strong>on</strong><br />

is slowed by incompatible informati<strong>on</strong> from a color word as<br />

opposed to a neutral c<strong>on</strong>trol. Hock and Egeth (1970) presented<br />

evidence from a short-term memory scanning task, which <strong>the</strong>y


saw as incompatible with a late, resp<strong>on</strong>se-c<strong>on</strong>flict account. Us-<br />

ing oral yes-no resp<strong>on</strong>ses (to avoid a semantic relati<strong>on</strong> with <strong>the</strong><br />

printed words), <strong>the</strong>y suggested that color-related words are rec-<br />

ognized earlier and <strong>the</strong>reby more likely to distract from encod-<br />

ing ink color.<br />

However, Dalrymple-Alford and Azkoul (1972) and Dyer<br />

(1973c) questi<strong>on</strong>ed <strong>the</strong>ir interpretati<strong>on</strong>, arguing that Hock and<br />

Egeth (1970) failed to distinguish between identificati<strong>on</strong> and<br />

covert naming, and that <strong>the</strong>ir c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong>s rested <strong>on</strong> accepting<br />

<strong>the</strong> null hypo<strong>the</strong>sis. The <strong>on</strong>ly o<strong>the</strong>r finding c<strong>on</strong>sistent with <strong>the</strong><br />

Hock and Egeth (1970) positi<strong>on</strong>--that <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Tecce and Dimartino<br />

0965) that words spoken at <strong>the</strong> time <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> ink-color encoding can<br />

ei<strong>the</strong>r facilitate or inhibit that encoding--was also criticized by<br />

Dyer (1973c, pp. 114-116)as relying <strong>on</strong> a questi<strong>on</strong>able assump-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong> about <strong>the</strong> rates <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing word versus color informa-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong>, The perceptual encoding account has not been very promi-<br />

nent since <strong>the</strong>n. At any rate, it would have trouble with several<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> key results in Appendix B (e.g., Results 7 and 14 am<strong>on</strong>g<br />

o<strong>the</strong>rs). Processing subsequent to encoding definitely seems to<br />

matter to <strong>the</strong> SWoop effect.<br />

Parallel Models. Accruing Evidence Toward a Decisi<strong>on</strong><br />

Virtually all earlier <strong>the</strong>ories <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> task were sequen-<br />

tial. Informati<strong>on</strong> was encoded from each dimensi<strong>on</strong> and <strong>the</strong>n<br />

analyzed, and <strong>the</strong>n a resp<strong>on</strong>se was produced, perhaps with an<br />

additi<strong>on</strong>al disambiguati<strong>on</strong> stage. Processing at <strong>on</strong>e stage had to<br />

be complete (or very close to complete) before <strong>the</strong> next stage<br />

could begin. Even if paraUel processing did appear in a model,<br />

it was limited to occurring within a stage; stage transiti<strong>on</strong>s were<br />

sequential. Thus, Mort<strong>on</strong> and Chambers (1973) saw processing<br />

within <strong>the</strong> identificati<strong>on</strong> stage as parallel, but transiti<strong>on</strong> to <strong>the</strong><br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se stage as sequential. Interference occurred at <strong>the</strong> entry<br />

to <strong>the</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>se stage.<br />

In recent years, models have begun to relax this strict sequen-<br />

tial emphasis, discarding <strong>the</strong> idea <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a limited-capacity re-<br />

sp<strong>on</strong>se stage (e.g., Eriksen & Schultz, 1979; Flowers & Wilcox,<br />

1982; Logan, 1980; McClelland & Rumelhart, 1981 ; Taylor,<br />

1977). Capacity limitati<strong>on</strong>s seen as system-resource limits (e.g.,<br />

Kahneman, 1973) lead to quite different c<strong>on</strong>cepti<strong>on</strong>s than did<br />

limitati<strong>on</strong>s seen as stage-specific bottlenecks (e.g., Broadbent,<br />

1958).<br />

Logan (1980, pp. 528-529) cast his model <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> effect<br />

as a decisi<strong>on</strong> process ga<strong>the</strong>ring evidence. Evidence accumulates<br />

over time until a resp<strong>on</strong>se threshold is reached; evidence from<br />

each dimensi<strong>on</strong> is processed at a rate governed by its weight.<br />

Two weights determine each dimensi<strong>on</strong>'s c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong> to <strong>the</strong><br />

decisi<strong>on</strong> (in terms <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> evidence): a stable, automatic weight and a<br />

flexible, strategic attenti<strong>on</strong>al weight. Total evidence at thresh-<br />

old is <strong>the</strong> sum <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> all evidence from all dimensi<strong>on</strong>s. If <strong>the</strong> evi-<br />

dence from o<strong>the</strong>r dimensi<strong>on</strong>s is c<strong>on</strong>sistent with <strong>the</strong> desired di-<br />

mensi<strong>on</strong>, this reduces <strong>the</strong> threshold and hence <strong>the</strong> processing<br />

time for <strong>the</strong> desired dimensi<strong>on</strong>. However, if irrelevant dimen-<br />

si<strong>on</strong>s provide evidence c<strong>on</strong>flicting with <strong>the</strong> desired dimensi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se speed will be slowed. The extent <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> intrusi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> an-<br />

o<strong>the</strong>r dimensi<strong>on</strong> will be a functi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> its weights; those with<br />

larger weights will have a greater impact <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> composite deci-<br />

si<strong>on</strong> process.<br />

Logan's model provides a reas<strong>on</strong>able prototype for such paral-<br />

THE STROOP EFFECT 191<br />

lel models. In general, evidence is accrued for a resp<strong>on</strong>se with-<br />

out requiring a "resp<strong>on</strong>se stage y Instead, interference arises<br />

because <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> priming received by competing re-<br />

sp<strong>on</strong>ses (i.e., <strong>the</strong> amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> evidence accrued for <strong>the</strong>m). This<br />

interference occurs during <strong>the</strong> ga<strong>the</strong>ring <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> evidence, a c<strong>on</strong>tinu-<br />

ous process. Some models assume independent evidence ga<strong>the</strong>r-<br />

ing al<strong>on</strong>g <strong>the</strong> dimensi<strong>on</strong>s, but many allow interacti<strong>on</strong> (e.g.,<br />

McClelland & Rumelhart, 1981; Taylor, 1977), greatly increas-<br />

ing <strong>the</strong> ability <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong>se models to capture data.<br />

Never<strong>the</strong>less, two problems arise in Logan's model (and pre-<br />

sumably in similar parallel models). First, <strong>the</strong> model appears to<br />

predict symmetrical facilitati<strong>on</strong> and interference; Logan cer-<br />

tainly portrayed <strong>the</strong> model that way in 1980.~° Yet facilitati<strong>on</strong> is<br />

virtually always substantially smaller than interference. Logan's<br />

model can accommodate this by assuming that equal changes<br />

in activati<strong>on</strong> for <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>gruent and inc<strong>on</strong>gruent c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s, rep-<br />

resented geometrically as equal angles <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> departure from <strong>the</strong><br />

neutral c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, intersect <strong>the</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>se threshold at different<br />

angles because <strong>the</strong> point <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> origin is not perpendicular to <strong>the</strong><br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se threshold (see, e.g., Ratcliff, 1978, Figure 4, p. 65).<br />

Note that such a model still must predict that <strong>the</strong> same manipu-<br />

lati<strong>on</strong>s will affect <strong>the</strong> magnitudes <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> both facilitati<strong>on</strong> and inter-<br />

ference, a predicti<strong>on</strong> definitely worthy <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> empirical test.<br />

The sec<strong>on</strong>d problem with Logan's model is familiar: Asym-<br />

metrical interference for color naming versus word reading is<br />

explained by differential time <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> availability <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> evidence<br />

from <strong>the</strong> two dimensi<strong>on</strong>s. As Logan (1980, p. 543) put it,<br />

"sources available so<strong>on</strong>er can influence (speeded) decisi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

about sources that become available later, but not vice versa:'<br />

This is tantamount to a relative speed-<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>-processing assump-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong>, which I have already argued against. How serious a prob-<br />

lem is this for this class <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> models?<br />

As it turns out, this is not a necessary c<strong>on</strong>sequence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> such<br />

models, particularly those allowing interacti<strong>on</strong> across dimen-<br />

si<strong>on</strong>s. To illustrate with <strong>the</strong> bulletin-board metaphor, <strong>the</strong> provi-<br />

si<strong>on</strong>al evidence from every dimensi<strong>on</strong> is c<strong>on</strong>tinuously updated<br />

<strong>on</strong> a bulletin board visible to all processors and studied by <strong>the</strong><br />

decisi<strong>on</strong> maker. Although <strong>the</strong> desired dimensi<strong>on</strong> may be closer<br />

to complete processing <strong>on</strong> a given trial, partial evidence still<br />

will be present from o<strong>the</strong>r dimensi<strong>on</strong>s and will c<strong>on</strong>tribute to<br />

<strong>the</strong> composite decisi<strong>on</strong>. Thus, even <strong>the</strong> "early returns" from a<br />

dimensi<strong>on</strong> being processed more slowly can influence output<br />

based <strong>on</strong> a dimensi<strong>on</strong> processed relatively quickly. There is still<br />

a speed element in this account (e.g., a very slow process might<br />

not return any evidence early enough to have any effect), but <strong>the</strong><br />

strict speed <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing assumpti<strong>on</strong> need not hold. Indeed,<br />

<strong>the</strong> variability in finishing times inherent in this stochastic rep-<br />

resentati<strong>on</strong> also permits deviati<strong>on</strong>s from strict speed <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> process-<br />

ing predicti<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Logan's model handles <strong>the</strong> major data in Appendix B quite<br />

well. He would expect many analogs <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> effect when<br />

multiple dimensi<strong>on</strong>s are being processed in parallel (Result 1).<br />

Because evidence would be collected at different levels <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> analy-<br />

sis, both orthographic (Result 2) and semantic (Result 3) influ-<br />

to J<strong>on</strong>athan Cohen, Kevin Dunbar, and Gord<strong>on</strong> Logan all indepen-<br />

dently pointed this out to me. The possible soluti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>fered here was<br />

provided in a letter from Gord<strong>on</strong> Logan.


192 COLIN M. MACLEOD<br />

ences should make <strong>the</strong>ir presence known. This implicates a<br />

greater role for <strong>the</strong> linguistic processing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> left hemisphere<br />

(Result 17), but without any anticipated sex differences (Re-<br />

sult 15).<br />

Prior activati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> some informati<strong>on</strong> should provide a head<br />

start to <strong>the</strong> evidence-ga<strong>the</strong>ring process, increasing <strong>the</strong> weight <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

that dimensi<strong>on</strong> (Result 4). Facilitati<strong>on</strong> follows naturally: Evi-<br />

dence collected from different dimensi<strong>on</strong>s points to <strong>the</strong> same<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se and makes <strong>the</strong> decisi<strong>on</strong> easier (Result 5). Perceptual<br />

integrati<strong>on</strong> should make it difficult to assign differential<br />

weights to dimensi<strong>on</strong>s, whereas separati<strong>on</strong> should make it eas-<br />

ier (Result 6). Mixing c<strong>on</strong>gruent and inc<strong>on</strong>gruent trials (Result<br />

7) should also disrupt <strong>the</strong> assigning <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> weights from trial to trial<br />

because sometimes it will be helpful to incorporate informa-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong> from o<strong>the</strong>r dimensi<strong>on</strong>s and sometimes it will be a hin-<br />

drance. The logic is similar for successive trial effects (Result 8)<br />

and for advance cueing (Result 9), both <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> which will cause<br />

temporary adjustments <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> dimensi<strong>on</strong>al weights.<br />

A reverse <strong>Stroop</strong> effect (Result I l) occurs when some manipu-<br />

lati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> usual situati<strong>on</strong> changes <strong>the</strong> directi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interfer-<br />

ence. In Logan's model, <strong>the</strong> relative weights <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> two dimen-<br />

si<strong>on</strong>s would also shift, as might <strong>the</strong> speed <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> ga<strong>the</strong>ring evidence<br />

for each. Practice (Result 12) should also affect <strong>the</strong> priority <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

each dimensi<strong>on</strong> and hence its weight. Once again, <strong>the</strong> develop-<br />

ment <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reading (Result 16) and <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> language ability (Result 18)<br />

can be seen as instances <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> naturally occurring practice. Be-<br />

cause evidence is being ga<strong>the</strong>red for <strong>on</strong>e <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a set <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> possible<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ses, <strong>the</strong> overlap <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> this set across <strong>the</strong> two or more dimen-<br />

si<strong>on</strong>s would be important (Result 14).<br />

Only <strong>the</strong> SOA and <strong>the</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>se modality effects do not flow<br />

easily from <strong>the</strong> Logan model, although <strong>the</strong>y can be handled.<br />

The word-reading task might not be affected by SOA (Result 10)<br />

because it is difficult to alter <strong>the</strong> weight <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> evidence drawn<br />

from <strong>the</strong> word; it is already highly weighted in <strong>the</strong> standard<br />

<strong>Stroop</strong> task. Apparent perceptual integrati<strong>on</strong> may also play a<br />

role in <strong>the</strong> largest interference being seen at <strong>the</strong> shortcst SOAs.<br />

As for resp<strong>on</strong>se mode (Result 13), less evidence may be required<br />

for a manual resp<strong>on</strong>se than for an oral <strong>on</strong>e, although this specu-<br />

lati<strong>on</strong> should be tested.<br />

With some fine tuning, Logan's model can encompass <strong>the</strong><br />

existing data. However, parallel models expressed <strong>on</strong>ly at <strong>the</strong><br />

c<strong>on</strong>ceptual level tend to have more "free parameters" than do<br />

sequential models, which may be part <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> why <strong>the</strong>y appear to bc<br />

more successful. What is required is a parallel model expressed<br />

formally, so that its predicti<strong>on</strong>s can be tested and evaluated.<br />

A Parallel Distributed Processing Model<br />

The most exciting recent development in models <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> cogniti<strong>on</strong><br />

is <strong>the</strong> parallel distributed processing framework (e.g., McClcl-<br />

land, 1979; Rumelhart, Hint<strong>on</strong>, & McClelland, 1986). Cohen,<br />

Dunbar, and McCleUand (1990) used this framework to build a<br />

model <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> effect. It incorporates many <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> virtues<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> automaticity and relative speed <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing but few <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong><br />

liabilities. Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, it is quite a natural extensi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong><br />

kinds <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> ideas embodied in <strong>the</strong> parallel models just described.<br />

At <strong>the</strong> core <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> Cohen et al, (1990) model is <strong>the</strong> idea that<br />

processing occurs in <strong>the</strong> system through activati<strong>on</strong> moving<br />

al<strong>on</strong>g pathways <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> different strength. C<strong>on</strong>sequently, relative<br />

speed-<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>-processing predicti<strong>on</strong>s need not always hold; it is<br />

strength, not speed, that is basic. Nor is str<strong>on</strong>g automaticity<br />

appropriate; a gradient <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> automaticity makes sense (cf. Logan,<br />

1985; MacLcod & Dunbar, 1988) wherein degree <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> automatic-<br />

ity is a functi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> strength <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> each pathway.<br />

Processing is performed in a system comprised <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interc<strong>on</strong>-<br />

nected modules. Within each module are c<strong>on</strong>tinuously operat-<br />

ing elementary processing units resp<strong>on</strong>sible for accepting in-<br />

puts from o<strong>the</strong>r units and providing output. Knowledge is repre-<br />

sented as a pattern <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> activati<strong>on</strong> over units, which can change<br />

with time in a c<strong>on</strong>tinuous, n<strong>on</strong>linear manner. Processing oc-<br />

curs by <strong>the</strong> spread <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> activati<strong>on</strong> al<strong>on</strong>g c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>s that exist<br />

both within modules and between modules. For simplificati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

Cohen ctal. (1990) assumed that informati<strong>on</strong> flows in <strong>on</strong>e direc-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong>-bottom up--from input to output.<br />

When <strong>the</strong> model is instructed to perform a task, it selects a<br />

pathway that includes some or all <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> units in <strong>on</strong>e or more<br />

modulcs. The set <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>s in this pathway specifies its<br />

strength, and <strong>the</strong> choice <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> pathway <strong>the</strong>refore determines both<br />

<strong>the</strong> speed and accuracy <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing. Individual units can be<br />

members <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> more than <strong>on</strong>e pathway, allowing interacti<strong>on</strong>s be-<br />

tween processes when <strong>the</strong>ir pathways intersect. Thus, if two<br />

pathways arc active simultaneously and produce c<strong>on</strong>flicting ac-<br />

tivati<strong>on</strong> at <strong>the</strong>ir intersecti<strong>on</strong>, interference results; if <strong>the</strong>y pro-<br />

duce coinciding activati<strong>on</strong>, facilitati<strong>on</strong> results. Such intersec-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong>s can occur anywhere in processing, and <strong>the</strong>re can be multi-<br />

ple intersecti<strong>on</strong>s. This realizes <strong>the</strong> essential details <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> Logan<br />

model. <strong>An</strong> important difference from Logan's model is <strong>the</strong> n<strong>on</strong>-<br />

linearity <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> processing units, permitting asymmetry be-<br />

tween facilitati<strong>on</strong> and interference.<br />

One <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> nice features <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> this model is its incorporati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a<br />

clear role for attenti<strong>on</strong>. Attenti<strong>on</strong> tunes, or modulates, <strong>the</strong> oper-<br />

ati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing units in a pathway. However, attenti<strong>on</strong> ac-<br />

complishes this tuning simply as ano<strong>the</strong>r source <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> informati<strong>on</strong><br />

would; it has no privileged status. This can be seen in Figure 2,<br />

which presents a minimalist view <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> model.<br />

In this simple case, <strong>the</strong>re are two pathways--<strong>on</strong>e for ink-color<br />

informati<strong>on</strong> and <strong>on</strong>e for word informati<strong>on</strong>--that share a re-<br />

Output Units<br />

Hidden Unite<br />

Input Units<br />

"red" "green"<br />

red greencolor Word red green<br />

INK COLOR \Naming Readingj. WORD<br />

Y<br />

TASK DEMANDS<br />

Figure 2. A parall¢! distributed processing model <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> effect<br />

(after Cohen, Dunbar, & McClel]and, 1990).


sp<strong>on</strong>se mechanism. Each pathway has a set <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> input units, each<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> which c<strong>on</strong>nects to every intermediate unit. In turn, each<br />

intermediate unit c<strong>on</strong>nects to all output units. Processing be-<br />

gins with <strong>the</strong> input units and feeds upward to <strong>the</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

units, <strong>on</strong>e <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> which will eventually accrue sufficient activati<strong>on</strong><br />

to exceed threshold and produce a resp<strong>on</strong>se. The <strong>on</strong>ly o<strong>the</strong>r<br />

element is <strong>the</strong> task-specific attenti<strong>on</strong>al units attached to <strong>the</strong><br />

task-appropriate intermediate units and capable <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> tuning at-<br />

tenti<strong>on</strong> by gating informati<strong>on</strong> flow.<br />

Cohen et al. (1990) represented each potential input and out-<br />

put as a single unit in <strong>the</strong> model, although <strong>the</strong>y pointed out that<br />

this <strong>on</strong>e-to-<strong>on</strong>e mapping is not essential. Like Logan's, <strong>the</strong><br />

model works by accruing evidence forward through <strong>the</strong> system,<br />

and <strong>the</strong> level <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> activati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a unit is a weighted sum <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong><br />

inputs reaching it. Learning occurs by changing c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong><br />

strengths based <strong>on</strong> minimizing mismatch between <strong>the</strong> desired<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se and <strong>the</strong> actual resp<strong>on</strong>se produced. Successive cycles,<br />

or trials, allow <strong>the</strong>se strength adjustments to occur. In this way,<br />

<strong>the</strong> network can be trained to produce particular resp<strong>on</strong>ses<br />

under particular sets <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> circumstances. Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

learning algorithm shows <strong>the</strong> power functi<strong>on</strong> widely seen in<br />

automaticity research.<br />

A resp<strong>on</strong>se occurs when a particular output unit's threshold<br />

is exceeded. A unit's resp<strong>on</strong>se strength is <strong>the</strong> ratio <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> its activa-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong> to total activati<strong>on</strong>. Attenti<strong>on</strong>, also a pattern <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> activati<strong>on</strong><br />

over some set <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> units, picks <strong>the</strong> pathway that determines <strong>the</strong><br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se, based <strong>on</strong> task instructi<strong>on</strong>s. Ordinarily, intermediate<br />

units rest near zero activati<strong>on</strong>, but relevant attenti<strong>on</strong>al c<strong>on</strong>nec-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong>s (stemming from task demands) push <strong>the</strong>se units toward a<br />

higher resting state, where <strong>the</strong>y are more resp<strong>on</strong>sive to inputs. It<br />

is through this kind <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> priming that attenti<strong>on</strong> tunes pathways.<br />

Cohen et al. (1990) simulated a number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> key results, includ-<br />

ing not <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>the</strong> basic <strong>Stroop</strong> asymmetry, but also <strong>the</strong> fact that<br />

interference usually exceeds facilitati<strong>on</strong> (e.g., Dunbar & Mac-<br />

Leod, 1984), <strong>the</strong> SPA effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Glaser and Glaser (1982), <strong>the</strong><br />

practice effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> MacLeod and Dunbar (1988), <strong>the</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>se-<br />

set effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Klein (1964), and o<strong>the</strong>r more general patterns such<br />

as <strong>the</strong> power law with practice (e.g., Logan, 1988) and a reas<strong>on</strong>-<br />

able resp<strong>on</strong>se-time distributi<strong>on</strong> (Ratcliff, 1978). Although <strong>the</strong>re<br />

are problems (e.g., when <strong>the</strong> word precedes <strong>the</strong> color and <strong>the</strong><br />

task is to name <strong>the</strong> color, <strong>the</strong> word locks in and produces mas-<br />

sive interference regardless <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> SOA), <strong>the</strong> model fares very well.<br />

What is particularly compelling is that <strong>the</strong>y set out to deal pri-<br />

marily with <strong>the</strong> MacLeod and Dunbar training studies, yet <strong>the</strong><br />

model does a fine job <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reproducing a diverse group <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> results,<br />

even in instances where speed <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing and str<strong>on</strong>g automa-<br />

ticity have made incorrect predicti<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

The Cohen et al. (1990) model provides a promising avenue<br />

for testing ideas about attenti<strong>on</strong>, and is eminently testable in its<br />

own right. As with <strong>the</strong> Logan model, all 18 <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> results in<br />

Appendix B are readily accommodated. All that is required are<br />

<strong>the</strong> noti<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> relative strength and attenti<strong>on</strong>al tuning <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

strength <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>s between units in various pathways,<br />

coupled with n<strong>on</strong>linearity <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing units. That it is a func-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong>ing model that makes explicit <strong>the</strong> underlying assumpti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

is surely a mark in its favor. Of course, it would be most c<strong>on</strong>vinc-<br />

ing to simulate all 18 critical results and to derive novel (and<br />

correct) predicti<strong>on</strong>s, but <strong>the</strong> model's initial success is still im-<br />

pressive. No doubt it will be put to more extensive test.<br />

THE STROOP EFFECT 193<br />

How different is <strong>the</strong> parallel distributed processing model<br />

from earlier explanati<strong>on</strong>s? My impressi<strong>on</strong> is that it amalga-<br />

mates <strong>the</strong> speed-<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>-processing and c<strong>on</strong>tinuum-<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>-automaticity<br />

ideas. Like Logan's account, it rests <strong>on</strong> parallel processing, but<br />

defeats <strong>the</strong> speed problem by emphasizing strength <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>nec-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong>s. It also makes clear that strength determines <strong>the</strong> extent <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

automaticity <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a process. Finally, it recognizes <strong>the</strong> importance<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> resp<strong>on</strong>se processes in <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> task without requiring a<br />

limited-capacity resp<strong>on</strong>se channel. In this way, <strong>the</strong> model suc-<br />

cessfully retains <strong>the</strong> powerful ideas <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> earlier explanati<strong>on</strong>s with-<br />

out being saddled with <strong>the</strong>ir flaws.l~<br />

The Final Word<br />

The first five secti<strong>on</strong>s set out <strong>the</strong> data base to be explained.<br />

Then <strong>the</strong> final secti<strong>on</strong> surveyed existing <strong>the</strong>ories, evaluating<br />

<strong>the</strong>m against this evidence. The two predominant <strong>on</strong>es--rela-<br />

tive speed <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing and automaticity--were seen as inade-<br />

quate. Ultimately, a new <strong>the</strong>oretical framework relying <strong>on</strong> paral-<br />

lel processing was c<strong>on</strong>sidered. This is a ra<strong>the</strong>rdifferent perspec-<br />

tive than <strong>the</strong> <strong>on</strong>e put forth by Dyer (1973c) that is still widely<br />

accepted. It will be interesting to see how such <strong>the</strong>ories fare<br />

when put to empirical test in upcoming years. I str<strong>on</strong>gly sus-<br />

pect that <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> effect will c<strong>on</strong>tinue to be a challenging<br />

phenomen<strong>on</strong> for cognitive psychologists to explain for many<br />

years to come. For now, I look forward to <strong>the</strong> progress that will<br />

be examined in <strong>the</strong> subsequent review <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> literature<br />

some time early in <strong>the</strong> next millennium.<br />

'J <strong>An</strong>o<strong>the</strong>r c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>ist model appeared subsequent to <strong>the</strong> final<br />

versi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> this review. Phaf, van der Heijden, and Huds<strong>on</strong> (1990) use an<br />

architectural property--direct c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>s between compatible stim-<br />

uli and resp<strong>on</strong>ses--instead <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> differential weights favored by Co-<br />

hen et al. (1990) to explain <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> effect.<br />

References<br />

Abramczyk, R. R., Jordan, D. E., & Hegel, M. (1983). "Reverse" <strong>Stroop</strong><br />

effect in <strong>the</strong> performance <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> schizophrenics. Perceptual and Motor<br />

Skills, 56, 99-106.<br />

Aine, C. J., & Harter, M. R. (1984a). Event-related potentials to <strong>Stroop</strong><br />

stimuli: Color and word processing. <strong>An</strong>nals <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> New York Academy<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Sciences, 425, 152-153.<br />

Aine, C. J., & Harter, M. R. (1984b). Hemispheric differences in event-<br />

related potentials to <strong>Stroop</strong> stimuli: Attenti<strong>on</strong> and color-word pro-<br />

cessing. <strong>An</strong>nals <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> New York Academy <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Sciences. 425, 154-156,<br />

Alivisatos, B., & Wilding, J. (1982). Hemispheric differences in match-<br />

ing <strong>Stroop</strong>-type letter stimuli. Cortex, 18, 5-21.<br />

Allport, D. A., Tipper, S. P., & Chmiel, N. R. J. (1985). Perceptual<br />

integrati<strong>on</strong> and postcategorical filtering. In M. I. Posner & O. S. M.<br />

Marin (Eds.), Attenti<strong>on</strong> and performance XI (pp. 107-132). Hillsdale,<br />

N J: Erlbaum.<br />

Alpers<strong>on</strong>, B. L. (1967). The effect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> semantic relatedness and practice <strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> color-word test. Unpublished doctoral dissertati<strong>on</strong>, Michigan<br />

State University, East Lansing.<br />

Alwitt, L. E (1966). Attenti<strong>on</strong> in a visual task am<strong>on</strong>g n<strong>on</strong>-readers and<br />

readers. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 23, 361-362.<br />

Arochova, O. (197 l). The use <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a modified <strong>Stroop</strong> Test in pre-school<br />

children. Psychologia a Patopsychologia Dietata, 6, 261-266. (From<br />

Psychological Abstracts, 1972, 48, Abstract No. 11282)<br />

Babbitt, K C. (1982). <strong>Effect</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> task demands <strong>on</strong> dual coding <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> pictorial


194 COLIN M. MACLEOD<br />

stimuli. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and<br />

Cogniti<strong>on</strong>, 8, 73-80.<br />

Bakan, P., & Alpers<strong>on</strong>, B. (1967). Pr<strong>on</strong>ounceability, attensity, and inter-<br />

ference in <strong>the</strong> color-word test. American Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Psychology, 80,<br />

416-420.<br />

Bakan, E, & Shotland, R. L. (1969). Lateral eye movement, reading<br />

speed, and visual attenti<strong>on</strong>. Psych<strong>on</strong>omic Science, 15, 93-94.<br />

Bartlett, E C. (1932). Remembering: A study in experimental and social<br />

psychology. New York: Macmillan.<br />

Bassett, J. E., & Schdlman, G. C. 0976). Performance <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> retardates <strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> color-word test. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 43, 1259-<br />

1262.<br />

Baumler, G. (1969). Decrease in achievement capacity as a result <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> age<br />

with particular reference to <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong>-interference tendency. Psy-<br />

chologische Beitrage, 11, 34-68. (From Psychological Abstracts,<br />

1970, 44, Abstract No. 10163)<br />

Beller, H. K. (1975). Naming, reading and executing directi<strong>on</strong>s. Jour-<br />

nal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psychology." Human Percepti<strong>on</strong> and Perfor-<br />

mance, 1, 154-160.<br />

Besner, D., Davies, J., & Daniels, S. (1981). Reading for meaning: The<br />

effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>current articulati<strong>on</strong>. Quarterly Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental<br />

Psychology, 33A, 415-437.<br />

Bettner, L. G., Jarvik, L. E, & Blum, J. E. (1971). <strong>Stroop</strong> color-word<br />

test, n<strong>on</strong>-psychotic organic brain syndrome, and chromosome loss<br />

in aged twins. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Ger<strong>on</strong>tology, 26, 458-469.<br />

Biederman, I., & Tsao, Y.-C. (1979). On processing Chinese ideographs<br />

and English words: Some implicati<strong>on</strong>s from <strong>Stroop</strong>-test results. Cog-<br />

nitive Psychology, 11, 125-132.<br />

Bills, A. G. (1931). Blocking: A new principle <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> mental fatigue. Ameri-<br />

can Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Psychology, 43, 230-245.<br />

B<strong>on</strong>e, R. N., & Eysenck, H. J. (1972). Extraversi<strong>on</strong>, field-dependence,<br />

and <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> test. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 34, 873-874.<br />

Boyden, J. G., & Gilpin, A. R. (1978). Matching familiar figures test<br />

and <strong>Stroop</strong> test performance in adults. Perceptual and Motor Skills,<br />

46, 854.<br />

Bradlyn, A. S., & Rollins, H. A. (1980). Incidental memory for <strong>the</strong><br />

color-word associati<strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> Color-Word Test. Bulletin <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong><br />

Psych<strong>on</strong>omic Society, 16, 269-272.<br />

Breslow, R., Grand, S., & Freedman, N. (1980). <strong>Effect</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> vocal feedback<br />

<strong>on</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> color-word interference. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 50,<br />

447--45 i.<br />

Brian, C. R., & Goodenough, E L. (1929). The relative potency <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> color<br />

and form percepti<strong>on</strong> at various ages. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psy-<br />

chology, 12, 197-213.<br />

Briggs, E, & Underwood, G. (1982). Ph<strong>on</strong>ological coding in good and<br />

poor readers. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Child Psychology, 34, 93-112.<br />

Broadbent, D. E. (1958). Percepti<strong>on</strong> and communicati<strong>on</strong>. L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong>: Per-<br />

gam<strong>on</strong> Press.<br />

Brown, W. (1915). Practice in associating color names with colors. Psy-<br />

chological <strong>Review</strong>, 22, 45-55.<br />

Brys<strong>on</strong>, S. E. (1983). Interference effects in autistic children: Evidence<br />

for <strong>the</strong> comprehensi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> single stimuli. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Abnormal Psy-<br />

chology, 92, 250-254.<br />

Butollo, W. H., Bauer, B., & Riedl, H. (1971). The equivalence to <strong>the</strong><br />

<strong>Stroop</strong> test for <strong>the</strong> preschool age? <strong>An</strong> experimental investigati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

interference tendency and its development in young children.<br />

Zeitschrift far Entwicklungpsychologie und Padagogische Psycholo-<br />

gie, 3, 181-194. (From Psychological Abstracts, 1972, 48, Abstract<br />

No. 622)<br />

Cammock, T., & Cairns, E. (1979). C<strong>on</strong>current validity <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a children's<br />

versi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> Color-Word Test: The fruit distracti<strong>on</strong> test.<br />

Perceptual and Motor Skills, 49, 611-616.<br />

Cattell, J. M. (1886). The time it takes to see and name objects. Mind,<br />

11, 63-65.<br />

Cheesman, J., & Merikle, E M. (1984). Priming with and without aware-<br />

ness. Percepti<strong>on</strong> and Psychophysics, 36, 387-395.<br />

Cheesman, J., & Merikle, E M. (1986). Distinguishing c<strong>on</strong>scious from<br />

unc<strong>on</strong>scious perceptual processes. Canadian Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Psychology,<br />

40, 343-367.<br />

Chen, H.-C., & Ho, C. (1986). Development <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>Stroop</strong> interference in<br />

Chinese-English bilinguals. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psychology:<br />

Learning, Memory, and Cogniti<strong>on</strong>, 12, 397-401.<br />

Chmiel, N. (1984). Ph<strong>on</strong>ological recoding for reading: The effect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

c<strong>on</strong>current articulati<strong>on</strong> in a <strong>Stroop</strong> task. British Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Psychol-<br />

ogy, 75, 213-220.<br />

Clark, H. H., & Browndl, H. H. (1975). Judging up and down. Journal<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psychology: Human Percepti<strong>on</strong> and Performance, 1,<br />

339-352.<br />

Cohen, G., & Martin, M. (1975). Hemisphere differences in an audi-<br />

tory <strong>Stroop</strong> task. Percepti<strong>on</strong> and Psychophysics, 17, 79-83.<br />

Cohen, J. D., Dunbar, K., & McClelland, J. L. (1990). On <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>trol <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

automatic processes: A parallel distributed processing account <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

<strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> effect. Psychological <strong>Review</strong>, 97, 332-361.<br />

Cohen, R., Meier, E., & Shulze, V. (1983). Sp<strong>on</strong>taneous reading in apha-<br />

sics (<strong>Stroop</strong>-test). der Nervenarzt, 54, 299-303.<br />

Cohn, N. B., Dustman, R. E., & Bradford, D. C. 0984). Age-related<br />

decrements in <strong>Stroop</strong> Color Test performance. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Clinical<br />

Psychology, 40, 1244-1250.<br />

Comalli, E E., Jr., Wapner, S., & Werner, H. (1962). Interference effects<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>Stroop</strong> color-word test in childhood, adulthood, and aging. Jour-<br />

nal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Genetic Psychology, 100, 47-53.<br />

Compt<strong>on</strong>, R. E, & Flowers, J. H. (1977). Pictorial interference with<br />

geometric shapes and achromatic shades. Percepti<strong>on</strong> and Psychophys-<br />

ics, 22, 303-309.<br />

C<strong>on</strong>rad, C. (1974). C<strong>on</strong>text effects in sentence comprehensi<strong>on</strong>: A study<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> subjective lexic<strong>on</strong>. Memory and Cogniti<strong>on</strong>, 2, 130-138.<br />

Corballis, M. C., & Lu<strong>the</strong>, L. (197 l). Two-channel visual memory. Per-<br />

cepti<strong>on</strong> and Psychophysics, 9, 361-367.<br />

Corbitt, J. R. (1978). Cognitive organizati<strong>on</strong> for words and colors as<br />

related to reading ability level: A developmental approach. (Doctoral<br />

dissertati<strong>on</strong>, University <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Wyoming, 1977) Dissertati<strong>on</strong> Abstracts<br />

Internati<strong>on</strong>al, 38, 4501-B.<br />

Cramer, E 0967). The <strong>Stroop</strong> effect in preschool aged children: A<br />

preliminary study. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Genetic Psychology, 111, 9-12.<br />

Dallas, M., & Merikle, E M. (1976). Semantic processing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> n<strong>on</strong>-at-<br />

tended visual informati<strong>on</strong>. Canadian Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Psychology, 30, 15-<br />

21.<br />

Dalrymple-Alford, E. C. (1968). Intedingual interference in a color-<br />

naming task. Psych<strong>on</strong>omic Science, 10, 215-216.<br />

Dalrymple-AIford, E. C. (1972a). Associative facilitati<strong>on</strong> and interfer-<br />

ence in <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> color-word task. Percepti<strong>on</strong> and Psychophysics, 11,<br />

274-276.<br />

Dalrymple-Alford, E. C. (1972b). Sound similarity and color word inter-<br />

ference in <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> task. Psych<strong>on</strong>omic Science, 28, 209-210.<br />

Dalrymple-Alford, E. C., & Azkoul, J. (1972). The locus <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interference<br />

in <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> and related tasks. Percepti<strong>on</strong> and Psychophysics, 11,<br />

385-388.<br />

Dalrymple-AIford, E. C., & Budayr, B. (1966). Examinati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> some<br />

aspects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> Color-Word Test. Perceptual and Motor Skills,<br />

23, 1211-1214.<br />

Daniel, J. (1968). Certain correlates in <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> interference test.<br />

Ceskoslovenska Psychologie, 12, 282-287.<br />

Daniel, J. (1969). Performance in an interference test and some<br />

changes in <strong>the</strong> vegetative functi<strong>on</strong>s. Studia Psychologica, 11, 267-<br />

271. (From Psychological Abstracts, 1970, 44, Abstract No. 2066 l)<br />

Daniel, J. (1970a). Achievement in <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> interference test in rela-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong> to pr<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>essi<strong>on</strong>. Studia Psychologica, 12. 99-102. (From Psycho-<br />

logical Abstracts, 1971, 45, Abstract No. 7107)


Daniel, J. (1970b). Fur<strong>the</strong>r variants <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>Stroop</strong>'s interference test. Studia<br />

Psychologica, 12, 80-8 I. (From Psychological Abstracts, 197 l, 45,<br />

Abstract No. 1658)<br />

Das, J. P. 0969). Development <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> verbal abilities in retarded and nor-<br />

mal children as measured by <strong>Stroop</strong> test. British Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Social<br />

and Clinical Psychology, 8, 59-66.<br />

Das, J. E (1970). Changes in <strong>Stroop</strong>-test resp<strong>on</strong>ses as a functi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

mental age. British Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Social and Clinical Psychology, 9, 68-<br />

73.<br />

Dash, J., & Dash, A. S. (1982). Cognitive developmental studies <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong><br />

<strong>Stroop</strong> phenomena: Cross-secti<strong>on</strong>al and l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal data. Indian<br />

Psychologist, 1, 24-33. (From Psychological Abstracts, 1984, 71, Ab-<br />

stract No. 9134)<br />

Davis, W., Jorgens<strong>on</strong>, C. B., Kritselis, A., & Opella, J. (1981). Hemi-<br />

spheric asymmetry in <strong>the</strong> processing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>Stroop</strong> stimuli: The effect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

enhancement <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> spatial skills. Internati<strong>on</strong>al Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Neuroscience,<br />

15, 179-183.<br />

Davis, W., Jorgens<strong>on</strong>, C. 13., & Opella, J. 0983). Hemispheric asym-<br />

metry in <strong>the</strong> processing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>Stroop</strong> stimuli: <strong>An</strong> altered format. Interna-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong>al Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Neuroscience, 19, 227-230.<br />

DeHaas, E A., & Young, R. D. 0984). Attenti<strong>on</strong> styles <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> hyperactive<br />

and normal girls. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Abnormal Child Psychology, 12, 531-<br />

545.<br />

Dennis, I., & Newstead, S. E. (1981). Is ph<strong>on</strong>ological recoding under<br />

strategic c<strong>on</strong>trol? Memory and Cogniti<strong>on</strong>, 9, 472--477.<br />

Doehrman, S., Landau, R., & O'C<strong>on</strong>nell, D. (1978). The <strong>Stroop</strong> phe-<br />

nomen<strong>on</strong>: Perceptual c<strong>on</strong>flict or resp<strong>on</strong>se competiti<strong>on</strong>? Perceptual<br />

and Motor Skills, 47, 1127-1131.<br />

Dornic, S. (1982). Semantic interference as a functi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> arousal. Re-<br />

ports from <strong>the</strong> Department <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Psychology, University <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Stockholm,<br />

No. 590, 1-8. (From Psychological Abstracts, 1984, 71, Abstract No.<br />

19521)<br />

Dornic, S., & Wirberg, A. (1983). Note <strong>on</strong> intralingual and interlingual<br />

interference in <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> task. Reports from <strong>the</strong> Department <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Psy-<br />

chology, University <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Stockholm, No. 611, 1-7. (From Psychological<br />

Abstracts, 1985, 72, Abstract No. 27143)<br />

Dosher, B. A., & Corbett, A. T. (1982). Instrument inferences and verb<br />

schemata. Memory and Cogniti<strong>on</strong>, 10, 531-539.<br />

Doten, G. W. (1955). The effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> rest periods <strong>on</strong> interference <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a<br />

well-established habit. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psychology, 49, 401 -<br />

406.<br />

Dunbar, K. N. (1986). Multiple sources <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interference in a picture-word<br />

analogue <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> task. Unpublished doctoral dissertati<strong>on</strong>, Uni-<br />

versity <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Tor<strong>on</strong>to.<br />

Dunbar, K. N., & MacLeod, C. M. (1984). A horse race <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a different<br />

color: <strong>Stroop</strong> interference patterns with transformed words. Journal<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psychology: Human Percepti<strong>on</strong> and Performance,<br />

10, 622-639.<br />

Duncan-Johns<strong>on</strong>, C. C., & KopeU, B. S. (1980). The locus <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interfer-<br />

ence in a <strong>Stroop</strong> task: When you read "blue" do you see "red"?<br />

Psychophysiology, 17, 308-309.<br />

Duncan-Johns<strong>on</strong>, C. C., & Kopell, B. S. (1981). The <strong>Stroop</strong> effect: Brain<br />

potentials localize <strong>the</strong> source <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interference. Science, 214, 938-940.<br />

Dyer, E N. (1971a). Color-naming interference in m<strong>on</strong>olinguals and<br />

bilinguals. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 10, 297-<br />

302.<br />

Dyer, E N. (1971b). A comparis<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> chromatic and achromatic ver-<br />

si<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> Color-Word Test. Psych<strong>on</strong>omic Science, 22, 235-<br />

237.<br />

Dyer, E N. (1971 c). The durati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> word meaning resp<strong>on</strong>ses: <strong>Stroop</strong><br />

interference for different preexposures <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> word. Psych<strong>on</strong>omic<br />

Science, 25, 229-231.<br />

Dyer, E N. (19-72). Latencies for movement naming with c<strong>on</strong>gruent and<br />

THE STROOP EFFECT 195<br />

inc<strong>on</strong>gruent word stimuli. Percepti<strong>on</strong> and Psychophysics, 11, 377-<br />

380.<br />

Dyer, E N. (1973a). Interference and facilitati<strong>on</strong> for color naming with<br />

separate bilateral presentati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> word and color. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

Experimental Psychology, 99, 314-317.<br />

Dyer, E N. (1973b). Same and different judgments for word-color pairs<br />

with "irrelevant" words or colors: Evidence for word-code compari-<br />

s<strong>on</strong>s. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psychology, 98, 102-108.<br />

Dyer, E N. (1973c). The <strong>Stroop</strong> phenomen<strong>on</strong> and its use in <strong>the</strong> study <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

perceptual, cognitive, and resp<strong>on</strong>se processes. Memory and Cogni-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong>, 1, 106-120.<br />

Dyer, E N. (1974). <strong>Stroop</strong> interference with l<strong>on</strong>g preexposures <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong><br />

word: Comparis<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> pure and mixed preexposure sequences. Bulle-<br />

tin <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> Psych<strong>on</strong>omic Society, 3, 8-10.<br />

Dyer, E N., & Severance, L. J. (1972). <strong>Effect</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> irrelevant colors <strong>on</strong><br />

reading <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> color names: A c<strong>on</strong>trolled replicati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> "reversed<br />

<strong>Stroop</strong>" effect. Psych<strong>on</strong>omic Science, 28, 336-338.<br />

Dyer, E N., & Severance, L. J. (1973). <strong>Stroop</strong> interference with succes-<br />

sive presentati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> separate inc<strong>on</strong>gruent words and colors. Journal<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psychology, 98, 438-439.<br />

Ettler, M. (1977a). Experimental c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong>s toward an analysis <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

<strong>the</strong> interference phenomen<strong>on</strong> observed with <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> test.<br />

Zeitschrift far Experimentelle und <strong>An</strong>gewandte Psychologie, 24, 244-<br />

281. (From Psychological Abstracts, 1978, 60, Abstract No. 2368)<br />

Effler, M. (1977b). The influence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> serial factors <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> test.<br />

Psychologische Beitrage, 19, 189-200. (From Psychological Ab-<br />

stracts, 1978, 60, Abstract No. 184)<br />

Effler, M. (1978a). Colour-distant versus colour-c<strong>on</strong>gruent colour<br />

words, <strong>the</strong>ir influence <strong>on</strong> naming times in <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong>-test. Psycholo-<br />

gische Beitrilge, 20, 345-359. (From Psychological Abstracts, 1980,<br />

64, Abstract No. 2591)<br />

Efller, M. (1978b). The influence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> similarity in names <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>Stroop</strong><br />

items. Archiv fur Psychologic, 131, 21-37. (From Psychological Ab-<br />

stracts, 1981, 65, Abstract No. 11873)<br />

Effler, M. (1980). Processes in naming <strong>Stroop</strong>-stimuli: <strong>An</strong> analysis<br />

with word repetiti<strong>on</strong> effects. Archiv far Psychologie, 133, 249-262.<br />

(From Psychological Abstracts, 1983, 69, Abstract No. 276)<br />

Emer, M. (1981). Interference by <strong>Stroop</strong> items depending <strong>on</strong> word<br />

frequency training and reacti<strong>on</strong> times <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> word comp<strong>on</strong>ents.<br />

Zeitschrift fur Experimentelle und <strong>An</strong>gewandte Psychologie, 28, 54-<br />

79. (From Psychological Abstracts, 1982, 67, Abstract No. 7459)<br />

Effler, M., & Rabenstein, E. (1979). Serial effects: A possibility for <strong>the</strong><br />

analysis <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> interference phenomen<strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong>-test? Psycho-<br />

logische Beitrage, 21, 417-438. (From Psychological Abstracts, 1981,<br />

65, Abstract No. 9227)<br />

Egeth, H. E., Blecker, D., & Kamlct, A. S. (1969). Verbal interference in<br />

a perceptual comparis<strong>on</strong> task. Percepti<strong>on</strong> and Psychophysics, 6, 355-<br />

356.<br />

Ehri, L. C. (1976). Do words really interfere in naming pictures? Child<br />

Development, 47, 502-505.<br />

Ehri, L. C. (1977). Do adjectives and functors interfere as much as<br />

nouns in naming pictures? Child Development, 48, 697-701.<br />

Ehri, L. C., & Ryan, E. B. (1980). Performance <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> bilinguals in a pic-<br />

ture-word interference task. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Psycholinguistic <str<strong>on</strong>g>Research</str<strong>on</strong>g>, 9,<br />

285-302.<br />

Ehri, L. C., & Wilce, L. S. (1979~ Does word training increase or de-<br />

crease interference in a <strong>Stroop</strong> task? Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Child<br />

Psychology, 27, 352-364.<br />

Ellis, N. R., Woodley-Zanthos, P., Dulaney, C. L., & Palmer, R. L.<br />

(1989). Automatic-effortful processing and cognitive inertia in per-<br />

s<strong>on</strong>s with mental retardati<strong>on</strong>. American Journal <strong>on</strong> Mental Retarda-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong>, 93, 412-423.<br />

Ellis<strong>on</strong>, A. E., & Lambert, W. E. (1968). Reducti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> resp<strong>on</strong>se interfer-


196 COLIN M. MACLEOD<br />

ence through verbal repetiti<strong>on</strong>. British Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Psychology, 59,<br />

147-155.<br />

Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). <strong>Effect</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> noise letters up<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

identificati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a target letter in a n<strong>on</strong>search task. Percepti<strong>on</strong> and<br />

Psychophysics, 16, 143-149.<br />

Eriksen, C. W., & H<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>fman, J. E. 0972). Temporal and spatial charac-<br />

teristics <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> selective encoding from visual displays. Percepti<strong>on</strong> and<br />

Psychophysics, 12, 201-204.<br />

Eriksen, C. W., & Schultz, D. W. (1979). Informati<strong>on</strong> processing in vi-<br />

sual search: A c<strong>on</strong>tinuous flow c<strong>on</strong>cepti<strong>on</strong> and experimental results.<br />

Percepti<strong>on</strong> and Psychophysics, 25. 249-263.<br />

Fang, S.-E, Tzeng, O. J., & Alva, L. (1981). Intralanguage vs. interlan-<br />

guage <strong>Stroop</strong> effects in two types <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> writing systems. Memory and<br />

Cogniti<strong>on</strong>, 9, 609-617.<br />

Fitts, E M., & Posner, M. I. (1967). Human performance. M<strong>on</strong>terey, CA:<br />

Brooks Cole.<br />

Flowers, J. H. (1975). "Sensory" interference in a word-color matching<br />

task. Percepti<strong>on</strong> and Psychophysics, 18, 37-43.<br />

Flowers, J. H., & Blair, B. (1976). Verbal interference with visual classifi-<br />

cati<strong>on</strong>: Optimal processing and experimental design. Bulletin <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong><br />

Psych<strong>on</strong>omic Society, 7, 260-262.<br />

Flowers, J. H., & Dutch, S. (1976). The use <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> visual and name codes in<br />

scanning and classifying colors. Memory and Cogniti<strong>on</strong>, 4, 384-390.<br />

Flowers, J. H., & Stoup, C. M. (1977). Selective attenti<strong>on</strong> between<br />

words, shapes and colors in speeded classificati<strong>on</strong> and vocalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

tasks. Memory and Cogniti<strong>on</strong>, 5, 299-307.<br />

Flowers, J. H., & Wilcox, N. 0982). The effect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> flanking c<strong>on</strong>text <strong>on</strong><br />

visual classificati<strong>on</strong>: The joint c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interacti<strong>on</strong>s at differ-<br />

ent processing levels. Percepti<strong>on</strong> and Psychophysics, 32, 581-59 I.<br />

Flowers, J. H., Warner, J. L., & Polansky, M. L. 0979). Resp<strong>on</strong>se and<br />

encoding factors in "ignoring" irrelevant informati<strong>on</strong>. Memory and<br />

Cogniti<strong>on</strong>, 7, 86-94.<br />

Fournier, E A., Mazzarella, M. M., Ricciardi, M. M., & Fingeret, A. L.<br />

0975). Reading level and locus <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interference in <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> color-<br />

word task. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 41, 239-242.<br />

Fox, L. A., Shor, R. E., & Steinman, R. J. (1971). Semantic gradients<br />

and interference in naming color, spatial directi<strong>on</strong>, and numerosity.<br />

Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psychology, 91, 59-65.<br />

Fraisse, E (1969). Why is naming l<strong>on</strong>ger than reading? Acta Psycho-<br />

logica, 30, 96-103.<br />

Francolini, C. M., & Egeth, H. E. (1980). On <strong>the</strong> n<strong>on</strong>-automaticity <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

"automatic" activati<strong>on</strong>: Evidence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> selective seeing. Percepti<strong>on</strong> and<br />

Psychophysics, 27, 331-342.<br />

Frey, E D. (1971). The effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> age, language, and reading ability <strong>on</strong><br />

color word interference under two c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> resp<strong>on</strong>ding. Unpub-<br />

lished doctoral dissertati<strong>on</strong>, Catholic University <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> America, Wash-<br />

ingt<strong>on</strong>, DC.<br />

Friedman, R. (197 l). The relati<strong>on</strong>ship between intelligence and perfor-<br />

mance <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> color-word test in sec<strong>on</strong>d- and fifth-grade chil-<br />

dren. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Genetic Psychology, 118, 147-148.<br />

Friedman, H., & Derks, E L. (1973). Simultaneous motor and verbal<br />

processing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> visual informati<strong>on</strong> in a modified <strong>Stroop</strong> test. Percep-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong> and Psychophysics, 13, 113-115.<br />

Gails, G. S. (1922). Individual differences as affected by practice. Ar-<br />

chiv fuer Psychologie, 8, 1-74.<br />

Gardner, R. M. (1985). The reverse affect test: A new interference task.<br />

Perceptual and Motor Skills, 60, 384-386.<br />

Gardner, R. W., Holzman, P. S., Klein, G. S., Lint<strong>on</strong>, H., & Spence, D. E<br />

(1959). Cognitive c<strong>on</strong>trol: A study <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> individual c<strong>on</strong>sistencies in cog-<br />

nitive behavior. Psychological Issues, 1, l-185.<br />

Gardner, R. W., & L<strong>on</strong>g, R. I. 0962). Cognitive c<strong>on</strong>trols <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> attenti<strong>on</strong><br />

and inhibiti<strong>on</strong>: A study <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> individual c<strong>on</strong>sistencies. British Journal<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

Psychology, 53, 381-388.<br />

Garner, W. R. (1974). The processing o<br />

fin formati<strong>on</strong> and structure. Hills-<br />

dale, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Garrett, H. E., & Lemm<strong>on</strong>, V. W. 0924). <strong>An</strong> analysis <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> several well-<br />

known tasks. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Applied Psychology, 8, 424-438.<br />

Gatti, S. V., & Egeth, H. E. (1978). Failure <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> spatial selectivity in visi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Bulletin <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> Psych<strong>on</strong>omic Society, I 1, 181-184.<br />

Ghols<strong>on</strong>, B., & Hohle, R. H. (1968a). Choice reacti<strong>on</strong> times to hues<br />

printed in c<strong>on</strong>flicting hue names and n<strong>on</strong>sense words. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

Experimental Psychology, 76, 413-.418.<br />

Ghols<strong>on</strong>, B., & Hohle, R. H. (1968b). Verbal reacti<strong>on</strong> times to hues vs<br />

hue names and forms vs form names. Percepti<strong>on</strong> and Psychophysics,<br />

3, 191-196.<br />

Glaser, M. O., & Glaser, W. R. (1982). Time course analysis <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong><br />

<strong>Stroop</strong> phenomen<strong>on</strong>. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psychology: Human<br />

Percepti<strong>on</strong> and Performance, 8, 875-894.<br />

Glaser, W. R., & Dolt, M. O. (1977). A functi<strong>on</strong>al model to localize <strong>the</strong><br />

c<strong>on</strong>flict underlying <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> phenomen<strong>on</strong>. Psychological Re-<br />

search, 39, 287-310.<br />

Glaser, W. R., & Dflngelh<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>f, E-J. 0984). The time course <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> picture-<br />

word interference. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psychology: Human Per-<br />

cepti<strong>on</strong> and Performance, 10, 640-654.<br />

Glaser, W. R., & Glaser, M. O. 0989). C<strong>on</strong>text effects in <strong>Stroop</strong>-like<br />

word and picture processing. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psychology:<br />

General, 118, 13-42.<br />

Golden, C. J. (1974a). <strong>Effect</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> differing number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>colors <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong><br />

Color and Word Test. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 39, 550.<br />

Golden, C. J. (1974b). Sex differences in performance <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong><br />

Color and Word Test. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 39, 1067-1070.<br />

Golden, C. J. 0975). A group versi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> Color and Word<br />

Test. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Pers<strong>on</strong>ality Assessment, 39, 386-388.<br />

Golink<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>f, R. M., & Rosinski, R. R. (1976). Decoding, semantic pro-<br />

cessing, and reading comprehensi<strong>on</strong> skill. Child Development, 47,<br />

252-258.<br />

Goolkasian, P. (1978). Reading and detecting color-word stimuli pre-<br />

sented at various retinal locati<strong>on</strong>s. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Psychology, 100, 167-<br />

181.<br />

Goolkasian, P. (1981). Retinal locati<strong>on</strong> and its effect <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> processing<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target and distractor informati<strong>on</strong>. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psy-<br />

chology: Human Percepti<strong>on</strong> and Performance, 7, 1247-1257.<br />

Grand, S., Breslow, R., & Freedman, N. 0980). On <strong>the</strong> role <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reduced<br />

auditory feedback and kinesic self-stimulati<strong>on</strong> during <strong>Stroop</strong> Color-<br />

Word performance. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Pers<strong>on</strong>ality, 48, 173-189.<br />

Green, E. J., & Barber, E J. (1981). <strong>An</strong> auditory <strong>Stroop</strong> effect with<br />

judgments <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> speaker gender. Percepti<strong>on</strong> and Psychophysics, 30,<br />

459-466.<br />

Green, E. J., & Barber, E J. (1983). Interference effects in an auditory<br />

<strong>Stroop</strong> task: C<strong>on</strong>gruence and corresp<strong>on</strong>dence. Acta Psychologica,<br />

53, 183-194.<br />

Greenwald, A. G. (1970). A double stimulati<strong>on</strong> test <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> ideomotor<br />

<strong>the</strong>ory with implicati<strong>on</strong>s for selective attenti<strong>on</strong>. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experi-<br />

mental Psychology, 84, 392-398.<br />

Greenwald, A. G. (1972). On doing two things at <strong>on</strong>ce: Time sharing as<br />

a functi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> ideomotor compatibility. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psy-<br />

chology, 94, 52-57.<br />

Grice, G. R., Boroughs, J. M., & Canham, L. (1984). Temporal dy-<br />

namics <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> associative interference and facilitati<strong>on</strong> produced by vi-<br />

sual c<strong>on</strong>text. Percepti<strong>on</strong> and Psychophysics, 36, 499-507.<br />

Grice, G. R., & Gwynne, J. W. (1985). Temporal characteristics <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> noise<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s producing facilitati<strong>on</strong> and interference. Percepti<strong>on</strong> and<br />

Psychophysics, 37, 495-501.<br />

Guiard, Y. (1981 ). <strong>Effect</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing mode <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> degree <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> motor<br />

asymmetry in <strong>the</strong> manual <strong>Stroop</strong> test. Cortex, 17, 427-433.<br />

Gumenik, W. E., & Glass, R. (1970). <strong>Effect</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reducing <strong>the</strong> readability


<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> words in <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> Color-Word Test. Psych<strong>on</strong>omic Science,<br />

20, 247-248.<br />

Guttentag, R. E., & Haith, M. M. (1978). Automatic processing as a<br />

functi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> age and reading ability. Child Development, 49, 707-716.<br />

Guttentag, R. E., Haith, M. M., Goodman, G. S., & Hauch, J. (1984).<br />

Semantic processing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> unattended words by bilinguals: A test <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong><br />

input switch mechanism. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Verbal Learning and Verbal Be-<br />

havior, 23, 178-188.<br />

Hall, R., & Swane, D. (1973). Memory in a bisensory selective attenti<strong>on</strong><br />

task. Quarterly Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psychology, 25, 309-315.<br />

Hamers, J. E-A. (1973). Interdependent and independent states <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong><br />

bifingual's two languages. Unpublished doctoral dissertati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

McGill University, M<strong>on</strong>treal, Quebec, Canada.<br />

Hamers, J. E, & Lambert, W. E. (1972). Bilingual interdependencies in<br />

auditory percepti<strong>on</strong>. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior,<br />

11, 303-310.<br />

Harbes<strong>on</strong>, M. M., Kennedy, R. S., & Bittner, A. C. (1981 ). A compari-<br />

s<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> test to o<strong>the</strong>r tasks for studies <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> envir<strong>on</strong>mental<br />

stress. US Naval Biodynamics Laboratory, 80R008, 20-28. (From<br />

Psychological Abstracts, 1983, 69, Abstract No. 7060)<br />

Harbes<strong>on</strong>, M. M., Krause, M., Kennedy, R. S., & Bittner, A. C. (1982).<br />

The <strong>Stroop</strong> as a performance evaluati<strong>on</strong> test for envir<strong>on</strong>mental re-<br />

search. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Psychology, 111, 223-233.<br />

Harms, L~ & Bundesen, C. (1983). Color segregati<strong>on</strong> and selective atten-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong> in a n<strong>on</strong>search task. Percepti<strong>on</strong> and Psychophysics, 33, 11-19.<br />

Harris<strong>on</strong>, N. S., & Boese, E. (1976). The locus <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> semantic interference<br />

in <strong>the</strong> "<strong>Stroop</strong>" color-naming task. Percepti<strong>on</strong> and Psychophysics, 20,<br />

408-412.<br />

Harvey, N. (1984). The <strong>Stroop</strong> effect: Failure to focus attenti<strong>on</strong> or fail-<br />

ure to maintain focusing? Quarterly Journal<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>Experimental Psychol-<br />

ogy: Human Experimental Psychology, 36, 89-115.<br />

Hatta, T. (1981). Differential processing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Kanji and Kana stimuli in<br />

Japanese people: Some implicati<strong>on</strong>s from <strong>Stroop</strong>-test results. Neu-<br />

ropsychologia, 19, 87-93.<br />

Hatta, T., & Kirsner, K. (1983). Language processing in partial En-<br />

glish-Japanese bilinguals: Evidence from <strong>Stroop</strong>-test results. Interna-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong>al Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Neuroscience, 20, 241-248.<br />

Hatta, T., Hatae, T. I., & Kirsner, K. (1984). Orthographic dominance<br />

and interference effects in letter recogniti<strong>on</strong> am<strong>on</strong>g Japanese-En-<br />

glish and English-Japanese bilinguals. Psychologia: <strong>An</strong> Internati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Psychology in <strong>the</strong> Orient, 27, 1-9. (From Psychological<br />

Abstracts, 1985, 72, Abstract No. 11353)<br />

Hatta, T., Katoh, H., & Aitani, N. (1983). Does single Kanji process<br />

dominantly in <strong>the</strong> right hemisphere? Some implicati<strong>on</strong> from <strong>Stroop</strong>-<br />

test results. Internati<strong>on</strong>al Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Neuroscience, 18, 67-72.<br />

Henik, A., Friedrich, E J., & Kellogg, W. A. (I 983). The dependence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

semantic relatedness effects up<strong>on</strong> prime processing. Memory and<br />

Cogniti<strong>on</strong>, 11, 366-373.<br />

Hentschel, U. (1973). Two new interference tests compared to <strong>the</strong><br />

<strong>Stroop</strong> Color-Word Test. Psychological <str<strong>on</strong>g>Research</str<strong>on</strong>g> Bulletin, Lund Uni-<br />

versity, 13, 1-24. (From Psychological Abstracts, 1974, 51, Abstract<br />

No. 8163)<br />

Hint<strong>on</strong>, W. M., Jr. (1976). The effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> number and spacing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> base<br />

item repetiti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> reacti<strong>on</strong> time to <strong>Stroop</strong>-like stimuli (Doctoral<br />

dissertati<strong>on</strong>, State University <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> New York at Buffalo, 1976). Disser-<br />

tati<strong>on</strong> Abstracts Internati<strong>on</strong>al, 37, 1005-B.<br />

Hintzman, D. L., Carte, E A., Eskridge, V. L., Owens, A. M., Shaft, S. S.,<br />

& Sparks, M. E. (1972). "<strong>Stroop</strong>" effect: Input or output phenome-<br />

n<strong>on</strong>? Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psychology, 95, 458--459.<br />

Hochman, S. H. (1973). The effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> drive level, n<strong>on</strong>-c<strong>on</strong>tingent suc-<br />

cess-failure feedback, and stimulus c<strong>on</strong>flictfulness <strong>on</strong> color-word<br />

performance (Doctoral dissertati<strong>on</strong>, City University <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> New York,<br />

1972). Dissertati<strong>on</strong> Abstracts Internati<strong>on</strong>al, 32, 668 I-B.<br />

Hock, H. S., & Egeth, H. (1970). Verbal interference with encoding in a<br />

THE STROOP EFFECT 197<br />

perceptual classificati<strong>on</strong> task. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psychology,<br />

83, 299-303.<br />

Hollingworth, H. L. (1912). Psychological aspects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> drug acti<strong>on</strong>. Psy-<br />

chological Bulletin, 9, 420--423.<br />

Hollingworth, H. L. (1915). Articulati<strong>on</strong> and associati<strong>on</strong>. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

Educati<strong>on</strong>al Psychology, 6, 99-105.<br />

Hollingworth, H. L. (1923). The influence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> alcohol. Journal<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>Abnor-<br />

mal and Social Psychology, 18, 204-237.<br />

Hunt, E., & Lansman, M. (1986). Unified model <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> attenti<strong>on</strong> and prob-<br />

lem solving. Psychological <strong>Review</strong>, 93, 446--461.<br />

Irwin, D. I., & Lupker, S. J. (1983). Semantic priming <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> pictures and<br />

words: A levels <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing approach. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Verbal Learning<br />

and Verbal Behavior, 22, 45-60.<br />

Irwin, H. J. (1978). Input encoding strategies and attenuati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>Stroop</strong><br />

interference. Australian Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Psychology, 30, 177-187.<br />

Jaensch, E. R. (1929). Grundformen menschlichen Seins. Berlin: Otto<br />

Elsner.<br />

James, W. (1890). The principles <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> psychology New York: Holt.<br />

Jarvik, L. E, Blum, J. E., & Varma, A. O. (1972). Genetic comp<strong>on</strong>ents<br />

and intellectual functi<strong>on</strong>ing during senescence: A 20-year study <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

aging twins. Behavior Genetics, 2, 159-171.<br />

Jensen, A. R. (1965). Scoring <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> test. Acta Psychologica, 24,<br />

398-408.<br />

Jensen, A. R., & Rohwer, W. D., Jr. (1966). The <strong>Stroop</strong> color-word test: A<br />

review. Acta Psychologica, 25, 36-93.<br />

Job, R., & Rumiati, R. (1984). <strong>Effect</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> degree <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> category member-<br />

ship in <strong>the</strong> figure-word interference task. Ricerche di Psicologia, 8,<br />

165-181. (From Psychological Abstracts, 1985, 72, Abstract No.<br />

11358)<br />

Johnst<strong>on</strong>, A., & Venables, E H. (1982). Specificity <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> attenti<strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong><br />

<strong>Stroop</strong> test: <strong>An</strong> EP study. Biological Psychology, 15, 75-83.<br />

J<strong>on</strong>ides, J., & Mack, R. (1984). On <strong>the</strong> cost and benefit <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> cost and<br />

benefit. Psychological Bulletin, 96, 29--44.<br />

Jorgens<strong>on</strong>, C., Davis, J., Opella, J., & <strong>An</strong>gerstein, G. (1980). Hemi-<br />

spheric-asymmetry in <strong>the</strong> processing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>Stroop</strong>-stimuli: <strong>An</strong> examina-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> gender, hand-preference, and language differences. Interna-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong>al Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Neuroscience, 11, 165-169.<br />

Jorgens<strong>on</strong>, C. B., Davis, J., Opella, J., & <strong>An</strong>gerstein, G. (1981). Hemi-<br />

spheric asymmetry in <strong>the</strong> processing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>Stroop</strong> stimuli: <strong>An</strong> examina-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> age and gender differences. Internati<strong>on</strong>al Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Neuro-<br />

science, 13, 113-116.<br />

Jorgens<strong>on</strong>, C. B., Davis, W E, Wilb<strong>on</strong>, P., & Opella, J. L. (1983). Hemi-<br />

spheric asymmetry in <strong>the</strong> processing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>Stroop</strong> stimuli: A balanced<br />

design. Internati<strong>on</strong>al Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Neuroscience, 21, 171-174.<br />

Kahneman, D. (1973). Attenti<strong>on</strong> andeffort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Pren-<br />

tice-Hall.<br />

Kahneman, D., & Chajczyk, D. (1983). Tests <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> automaticity <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

reading: Diluti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>Stroop</strong> effects by color-irrelevant stimuli. Jour-<br />

nal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psychology: Human Percepti<strong>on</strong> and Perfor-<br />

mance, 9, 497-509.<br />

Kahneman, D., & Henik, A. (1981 ). Perceptual organizati<strong>on</strong> and atten-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong>. In M. Kubovy & J. R. Pomerantz (Eds.), Perceptual organiza-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong> (pp. 181-211 ). Hillsdale, N J: Erlbaum.<br />

Kalk<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>en, H. (1969). Critical investigati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> interference phenome-<br />

n<strong>on</strong> (STROOP). Unpublished doctoral dissertati<strong>on</strong>, University <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

Carolo-Wilhelmina, Braunschweig.<br />

Kalk<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>en, H. (1970). Change in degree <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interference in a <strong>Stroop</strong>-like<br />

test (SWT) when <strong>the</strong> inc<strong>on</strong>gruent combinati<strong>on</strong>s aremixed with c<strong>on</strong>-<br />

gruent <strong>on</strong>es: A reply to Pr<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>. G. J. W. Smith. Psychologische For-<br />

schung, 34, 97-98. (From PsychologicalAbstracts, 1971,46, Abstract<br />

No. 355)<br />

Kamlet, A. S., & Egeth, H. E. (1969). Note <strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>structi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>Stroop</strong>-<br />

type stimuli. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 29, 914.<br />

Kareev, Y. (1980). Developmental changes in <strong>the</strong> internal encoding <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>


198 COLIN M. MACLEOD<br />

sentences as measured by <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> technique. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Verbal<br />

Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 549-561.<br />

Kareev, Y. (1982). Minitypologies from within-subjects designs: Uncov-<br />

ering systematic individual differences in experiments. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 21, 363-382.<br />

Keele, S. (1972). Attenti<strong>on</strong> demands <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> memory retrieval. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

Experimental Psychology, 93, 245-248.<br />

Keenan, J. M., Potts, G. R., Jennings, 1". M., & Golding, J. M. (1988,<br />

November). Can naming detect inferences? Paper presented at <strong>the</strong><br />

annual meeting <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> Psych<strong>on</strong>omic Society, Chicago, IL.<br />

Kinchla, R. A., Solis-Macias, V., & H<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>fman, J. (1983). Attending to<br />

different levels <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> structure in a visual image. Percepti<strong>on</strong> and Psycho-<br />

physics, 33, 1-10.<br />

Kipnis, D., & Glickman, A. S. (! 958). The development <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a n<strong>on</strong>-cogni-<br />

tive battery to predict enlisted performance. USN Bureau <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Naval<br />

Pers<strong>on</strong>nel Technical Bulletin, 58-9. (From Psychological Abstracts,<br />

1960, 34, Abstract No. 2233)<br />

Kipnis, D., & Glickman, A. S. (1959a). The development <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a n<strong>on</strong>-cog-<br />

nitive battery: Predicti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>radioman performance. USN Bureau <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

Naval Pers<strong>on</strong>nel Technical Bulletin, 59-14. (From Psychological Ab-<br />

stracts, 1960, 34, Abstract No. 3475)<br />

Kipnis, D., & Giickman, A. S. (1959b). Validity <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> n<strong>on</strong>-cognitive tests at<br />

Nuclear Power School (submarine). USN Bureau <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Naval Pers<strong>on</strong>nel<br />

Technical Bulletin, 59-6. (From Psychological Abstracts, 1960, 34,<br />

Abstract No. 3551)<br />

Kipnis, D., & Glickman, A. S. (1962). The predicti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> job perfor-<br />

mance. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Applied Psychology, 46, 50-56.<br />

Kiyak, H. A. (1982). Interlingual interference in naming color words.<br />

Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Cross-Cultural Psychology, 13, 125-135.<br />

Klein, G. S. (1964). Semantic power measured through <strong>the</strong> interference<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words with color-naming. American Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Psychology, 77,<br />

576-588.<br />

LaBerge, D., & Samuels, S. J. (1974). Toward a <strong>the</strong>ory <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> automatic<br />

informati<strong>on</strong> processing in reading. Cognitive Psychology, 6, 293-<br />

323.<br />

La Heij, W. (1988). Comp<strong>on</strong>ents <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>Stroop</strong>-like interference in picture<br />

naming. Memory and Cogniti<strong>on</strong>, 16, 400-4 10.<br />

La Heij, W., Van der Heijden, A. H. C., & Schreuder, R. (1985). Seman-<br />

tic priming and <strong>Stroop</strong>-like interference in word-naming tasks. Jour-<br />

nal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psychology: Human Percepti<strong>on</strong> and Perfor-<br />

mance, 11, 62-80.<br />

La Heij, W, & Vermeij, M. (1987). Reading versus naming: The effect<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> target set size <strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>textual interference and facilitati<strong>on</strong>. Percep-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong> and Psychophysics, 41, 355-366.<br />

Langer, J., & Rosenberg, B. G. (1966). Symbolic meaning and color<br />

naming. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Pers<strong>on</strong>ality and Social Psychology, 4, 364-373.<br />

Langlois, J. (1974). Frequency <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> occurrence as a factor in interference<br />

<strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> word-color test. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 38, 986.<br />

Lassen, G. L. (1975). Extensi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> interference effect to<br />

pictures and words (Doctoral dissertati<strong>on</strong>, University <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Ariz<strong>on</strong>a,<br />

1974). Dissertati<strong>on</strong> Abstracts Internati<strong>on</strong>al, 35, 0223-B.<br />

Lig<strong>on</strong>, E. M. (1932). A genetic study <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> color naming and word reading.<br />

American Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Psychology, 44, 103-122.<br />

Liu, A.-Y. (1973). Decrease in <strong>Stroop</strong> effect by reducing semantic inter-<br />

ference. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 37, 263-265.<br />

Logan, G. D. (1978). Attenti<strong>on</strong> in character-classificati<strong>on</strong> tasks: Evi-<br />

dence for <strong>the</strong> automaticity <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> comp<strong>on</strong>ent stages. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experi-<br />

mental Psychology: General, 107, 32-63.<br />

Logan, G. D. (1980). Attenti<strong>on</strong> and automaticity in <strong>Stroop</strong> and priming<br />

tasks: Theory and data. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 523-553.<br />

Logan, G. D. (1985). Skill and automaticity: Relati<strong>on</strong>s, implicati<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

and future directi<strong>on</strong>s. Canadian Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Psychology, 39, 367-386.<br />

Logan, G. D. (1988). Toward an instance <strong>the</strong>ory <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> automatizati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Psychological <strong>Review</strong>, 95, 492-527.<br />

Logan, G. D., & Zbrod<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>f, N. J. (1979). When it helps to be misled:<br />

Facilitative effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> increasing <strong>the</strong> frequency <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>flicting stimuli<br />

in a <strong>Stroop</strong>-like task. Memory and Cogniti<strong>on</strong>, 7, 166-174.<br />

Logan, G. D., & Zbrod<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>f, N. J. (1982). C<strong>on</strong>straints <strong>on</strong> strategy c<strong>on</strong>-<br />

structi<strong>on</strong> in a speeded discriminati<strong>on</strong> task. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental<br />

Psychology." Human Percepti<strong>on</strong> and Performance, 8, 502-520.<br />

Logan, G. D., Zbrod<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>f, N. J., & Williams<strong>on</strong>, J. (1984). Strategies in <strong>the</strong><br />

color-word <strong>Stroop</strong> task. Bulletin <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> Psych<strong>on</strong>omic Society, 22, 135-<br />

138.<br />

L<strong>on</strong>g, (3. M., & Lyman, B. J. (1987). Foveal and parafoveal processing<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> asynchr<strong>on</strong>ous <strong>Stroop</strong> stimuli. British Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Psychology, 78,<br />

151-162.<br />

Lowe, D. G. (1979). Strategies, c<strong>on</strong>text, and <strong>the</strong> mechanism <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

inhibiti<strong>on</strong>. Memory and Cogniti<strong>on</strong>, 7, 382-389.<br />

Lowe, D. G. (1985). Fur<strong>the</strong>r investigati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> inhibitory mechanisms in<br />

attenti<strong>on</strong>. Memory and Cogniti<strong>on</strong>, 13, 74-80.<br />

Lowe, D. G., & Mitterer, J. O. (1982). Selective and divided attenti<strong>on</strong> in a<br />

<strong>Stroop</strong> task. Canadian Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Psychology, 36, 684-700.<br />

Ludwig, R. E, & Lazarus, E J. (1983). Relati<strong>on</strong>ship between shyness in<br />

children and c<strong>on</strong>stricted cognitive c<strong>on</strong>trol as measured by <strong>the</strong><br />

<strong>Stroop</strong> Color-Word Test. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> C<strong>on</strong>sulting and Clinical Psychol-<br />

ogy, 51, 386-389.<br />

Lund, E H. (1927). The role <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> practice in speed <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> associati<strong>on</strong>. Journal<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psychology, 10, 424-434.<br />

Lupker, S. J. (1979). The semantic nature <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> resp<strong>on</strong>se competiti<strong>on</strong> in<br />

<strong>the</strong> picture-word interference task. Memory and Cogniti<strong>on</strong>, 7, 485-<br />

495.<br />

Lupker, S. J. (1982). The role <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> ph<strong>on</strong>etic and orthographic similarity in<br />

picture-word interference. Canadian Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Psychology, 36, 349-<br />

367.<br />

Lupker, S. J., & Katz, A. N. (1981). Input, decisi<strong>on</strong>, and resp<strong>on</strong>se factors<br />

in picture-word interference. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psychology:<br />

Human Learning and Memory, 7, 269-282.<br />

Lupker, S. J., & Katz, A. N. (1982). Can automatic picture processing<br />

influence word judgments? Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psychology."<br />

Learning, Memory, and Cogniti<strong>on</strong>, 8, 418-434.<br />

Lupker, S. J., & Sanders, M. (1982). Visual-field differences in picture-<br />

word interference. Brain and Cogniti<strong>on</strong>, 1, 381-398.<br />

Maccoby, E. M., & Jacklin, C. N. (1974). The psychology <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> sex differ-<br />

ences. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.<br />

MacKinn<strong>on</strong>, D. E, Geiselman, R. E., & Woodward, J. A. (1985). The<br />

effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> effort <strong>on</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> interference. Acta Psychologica, 58, 225-<br />

235.<br />

MacLeod, C. M. (1986). [A replicati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> classic <strong>Stroop</strong> effect].<br />

Unpublished raw data.<br />

MacLeod, C. M. (in press). John Ridley <strong>Stroop</strong>: Creator <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a landmark<br />

cognitive task. Canadian Psychology.<br />

MacLeod, C. M., & Dunbar, K. (1988). Training and <strong>Stroop</strong>-like inter-<br />

ference: Evidence for a c<strong>on</strong>tinuum o fautomaticity. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experi-<br />

mental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cogniti<strong>on</strong>, 14, 126-135.<br />

Magee, L. (1982). Pictures, words, and access to informati<strong>on</strong>. Unpub-<br />

lished doctoral dissertati<strong>on</strong>, University <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Tor<strong>on</strong>to.<br />

M[igiste, E. (1984). <strong>Stroop</strong> tasks and dichotic translati<strong>on</strong>: The develop-<br />

ment <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interference patterns in bilinguals. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental<br />

Psychology." Learning, Memory, and Cogniti<strong>on</strong>, 10, 304-315.<br />

M~lgiste, E. (1985). Development <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>intra-and inteflingual interference<br />

in bilinguals. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Psycholinguistic <str<strong>on</strong>g>Research</str<strong>on</strong>g>, 14, 137-154.<br />

Majeres, R. L. (1974). The combined effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> stimulus and resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> delay in identifying <strong>the</strong> print color <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words. Jour-<br />

nal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psychology, 102, 868-874.<br />

Marcel, A. J. (1983). C<strong>on</strong>scious and unc<strong>on</strong>scious percepti<strong>on</strong>: Experi-<br />

ments <strong>on</strong> visual masking and word recogniti<strong>on</strong>. Cognitive Psychol-<br />

ogy, 15, 197-237.


Martin, M. (1978). Speech recoding in silent reading. Memory and<br />

Cogniti<strong>on</strong>, 6, 108-114.<br />

Martin, M. (1979)..Local and global processing: The role <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> sparsity.<br />

Memory and Cogniti<strong>on</strong>, 7, 476-484.<br />

Martin, M. (! 981). Reverse <strong>Stroop</strong> effect with c<strong>on</strong>current tasks. Bulle-<br />

tin <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> Psych<strong>on</strong>omic Society, 17, 8-9.<br />

McClain, L. (1983a). Color priming affects <strong>Stroop</strong> interference. Percep-<br />

tual and Motor Skills, 56, 643-651.<br />

McClain, L. (1983b). <strong>Effect</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> resp<strong>on</strong>se type and set size <strong>on</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong><br />

color-word performance. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 56, 735-743.<br />

McClain, L. (1983c). Stimulus-resp<strong>on</strong>se compatibility affects auditory<br />

<strong>Stroop</strong> interference. Percepti<strong>on</strong> and Psychophysics, 33, 266-270.<br />

McClelland, J. L. (1979). On <strong>the</strong> time relati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> mental processes: <strong>An</strong><br />

examinati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> systems <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processes in cascade. Psychological Re-<br />

view, 86, 287-330.<br />

McClelland, J. L., & Rumelhart, D. E. (1981). <strong>An</strong> interactive activati<strong>on</strong><br />

model <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>text effects in letter percepti<strong>on</strong>: Part 1. <strong>An</strong> account <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

basic findings. Psychological <strong>Review</strong>, 88, 375-407.<br />

McCown, D., & Arnoult, M. D. (1981). Interference produced by modi-<br />

fied <strong>Stroop</strong> stimuli. Bulletin <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> Psych<strong>on</strong>omic Society, 17, 5-7.<br />

M6nard-Buteau, C., & Cavanagl h P. (1984). Localizati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> form/<br />

colour interference at <strong>the</strong> perceiatual level in a <strong>Stroop</strong> task with stim-<br />

uli drawings. Canadian Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Psychology, 38, 421 --439.<br />

Merikle, P. M., & Gorewich, N. J. (1979). Spatial selectivity in visi<strong>on</strong>:<br />

Field size depends up<strong>on</strong> noise size. Bulletin <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> Psych<strong>on</strong>omic Soci-<br />

ety, 14, 343-346.<br />

Merrill, E. C., Sperber, R. D., & McCauley, :C. (1981). Differences in<br />

semantic encoding as a functi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reading comprehensi<strong>on</strong> skill.<br />

Memory and Cogniti<strong>on</strong>, 9, 618-624.<br />

Meyer, D. E., & Schvaneveldt, R. W. (1976). Meaning, memory struc-<br />

ture, and mental processes. Science, 192, 27-33.<br />

Morikawa, Y. (1981). <strong>Stroop</strong> phenomena in <strong>the</strong> Japanese language:<br />

The case <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> ideographic characters (kanji) and syllabic characters<br />

(kana). Perceptual and Motor Skills, 53, 67-77.<br />

Mort<strong>on</strong>, J. (1969). Categories <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> i~lerference: Verbal mediati<strong>on</strong> and<br />

c<strong>on</strong>flict in card sorting. British Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Psychology, 60, 329-346.<br />

Mort<strong>on</strong>, J., & Chambers, S. M. (1973). Selective attenti<strong>on</strong> to words and<br />

colours. Quarterly Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psychology, 25, 387-397.<br />

Murray, D. J., Mastr<strong>on</strong>ardi, J., & Duncan, S. (1972). Selective attenti<strong>on</strong><br />

to "physical" vs "verbal"/ispects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> colored words. Psych<strong>on</strong>omic<br />

Science, 26, 305-307.<br />

Naish, P. (1980). Ph<strong>on</strong>ological recoding and <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> effect. British<br />

Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Psychology, 71, 395-400.<br />

Nav<strong>on</strong>, D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> global fea-<br />

tures in visual percepti<strong>on</strong>. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 353-383.<br />

Nealis, P. M. (1973). The <strong>Stroop</strong> phenomen<strong>on</strong>: Some critical tests <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

<strong>the</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>se competiti<strong>on</strong> hypo<strong>the</strong>sis. Perceptual and Motor Skills,<br />

37, 147-153.<br />

Nealis, P. M. (1974). Reversal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>Stroop</strong> test: Interference in word read-<br />

ing. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 38, 379-382.<br />

Neill, W. T (1977). Inhibitory and facilitatory processes in selective<br />

attenti<strong>on</strong>. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psychology." Human Percepti<strong>on</strong><br />

and Performance, 3, 444-450.<br />

Neill, W. T (1978). Decisi<strong>on</strong> processes in selective attenti<strong>on</strong>: Resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

priming in <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> color-word task. Percepti<strong>on</strong> and Psychophys-<br />

ics, 23, 80-84.<br />

Neill, W. T., & Westberry, R. L. (1987). Selective attenti<strong>on</strong> and <strong>the</strong><br />

suppressi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> cognitive noise. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psychology:<br />

Learning, Memory, and Cogniti<strong>on</strong>, 13, 327-334.<br />

Neumann, O. (1980). Selecti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> informati<strong>on</strong> and c<strong>on</strong>trol <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> acti<strong>on</strong>. Un-<br />

published doctoral dissertati<strong>on</strong>, University <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Bochum, Federal Re-<br />

public <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Germany.<br />

Newman, J. P, (1981). Induced anxiety and <strong>Stroop</strong> Color-Word Test<br />

THE STROOP EFFECT 199<br />

performance: Hemispheric processing implicati<strong>on</strong>s. Internati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Neuroscience, 12, 63-66.<br />

Nielsen, G. D. (1975). The locus and mechanism <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> color<br />

word effect (Doctoral dissertati<strong>on</strong>, University <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Wisc<strong>on</strong>sin--Mad-<br />

is<strong>on</strong>, 1974). Dissertati<strong>on</strong> Abstracts Internati<strong>on</strong>al, 35, 5672-B.<br />

Oden, G. C., & Spira, J. L. (1983). Influence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>text <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> activati<strong>on</strong><br />

and selecti<strong>on</strong> o f ambiguous word senses. Quarterly Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experi-<br />

mental Psychology, 35.4, 51-64.<br />

Ogura, C. (1980). Formati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> learning-set in <strong>the</strong> task <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>Stroop</strong> color-<br />

word test. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Child Development, 16, 30-36.<br />

Palef, S. R. (1978). Judging pictorial and linguistic aspects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> space.<br />

Memory and Cogniti<strong>on</strong>, 6, 70-75.<br />

Palef, S. R., & Ols<strong>on</strong>, D. R. (1975). Spatial and verbal rivalry in a<br />

<strong>Stroop</strong>-like task. Canadian Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Psychology, 29, 201-209.<br />

Panck, P. E., Rush, M. C., & Slade, L. A. (1984). Focus <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> age-<strong>Stroop</strong><br />

interference relati<strong>on</strong>ship. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Genetic Psychology, 145, 209-<br />

216.<br />

Peretti, E O. (1969). Cross-sex and cross-educati<strong>on</strong>al level perfor-<br />

mance in a color-word interference task. Psych<strong>on</strong>omic Science, 16,<br />

321-323.<br />

Peretti, P. O. (197 l). <strong>Effect</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> n<strong>on</strong>competitive, competitive instruc-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong>s, and sex <strong>on</strong> performance in a color-word interference task.<br />

Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Psychology, 79, 67-70.<br />

Perret, E. (1974). The left fr<strong>on</strong>tal lobe <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> man and <strong>the</strong> suppressi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

habitual resp<strong>on</strong>ses in verbal categorical behaviour. Neuropsycholo-<br />

gia, 12, 323-330.<br />

Peters<strong>on</strong>, J., Lanier, L. H., & Walker, H. M. (1925). Comparis<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

white and negro children in certain ingenuity and speed tests. Jour-<br />

nal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Comparative Psychology, 5, 271-283.<br />

Phaf, R. H., Van der Heijden, A. H. C., & Huds<strong>on</strong>, P. T. W. (1990).<br />

SLAM: A c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>ist model for attenti<strong>on</strong> in visual selecti<strong>on</strong> tasks.<br />

Cognitive Psychology, 22, 273-341.<br />

Pieters, J. M. (198 l). Ear asymmetry in an auditory spatial <strong>Stroop</strong> task<br />

as a functi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> handedness. Cortex, 17, 369-379.<br />

Podell, H. A. (1963). Note <strong>on</strong> successive dimensi<strong>on</strong>al analysis applied<br />

to affective, cognitive, and pers<strong>on</strong>ality traits. Psychological Reports,<br />

13, 813-814.<br />

Posnansky, C. J., & Rayner, K. 0977). Visual-feature and resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

comp<strong>on</strong>ents in a picture-word interference task with beginning and<br />

skilled readers. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Child Psychology, 24, 440-<br />

460.<br />

Posnansky, C. J., & Rayner, K. (1978). Visual vs. ph<strong>on</strong>emic c<strong>on</strong>tribu-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong>s to <strong>the</strong> importance <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> initial letter in word identificati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Bulletin <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> Psych<strong>on</strong>omic Society, 11, 188-190.<br />

Posner, M. I. 0978). Chr<strong>on</strong>ometric explorati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> mind. Hillsdale, NJ:<br />

Erlbaum.<br />

Posner, M. I., & Snyder, C. R. R. 0975). Attenti<strong>on</strong> and cognitive c<strong>on</strong>-<br />

trol. In R. L. Solso (Ed.), Informati<strong>on</strong> processing and cogniti<strong>on</strong>: The<br />

Loyola symposium (pp. 55-85). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Posner, M. I., Walker, J. A., Friedrich, E J., & Rafal, R. D. (1984).<br />

<strong>Effect</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> parietal injury <strong>on</strong> covert orienting <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> attenti<strong>on</strong>. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

Neuroscience, 4, 1863-1874.<br />

Prest<strong>on</strong>, M. S., & Lambert, W E. (1969). Intcrlingual interference in a<br />

bilingual versi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> color-word task. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Verbal<br />

Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8, 295-301.<br />

Pritchard, W (198 l). Psychophysiology <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> P300. Psychological Bulle-<br />

tin, 89, 506-540.<br />

Pritchatt, D. (1968). <strong>An</strong> investigati<strong>on</strong> into some <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> underlying asso-<br />

ciative verbal processes <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> colour effect. Quarterly Journal<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psychology, 20, 351-359.<br />

Proctor, R. W. (1978). Sources <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> color-word interference in <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong><br />

color-naming task. Percepti<strong>on</strong> and Psychophysics, 23, 413-419.<br />

Quantz, J. O. (1897). Problems in <strong>the</strong> psychology <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reading. Psychologi-<br />

cal <strong>Review</strong> M<strong>on</strong>ographs, 2(No. 5).


200 COLIN M. MACLEOD<br />

Rand, G., Wapner, S., Werner, H., & McFarland, J. H. (1963). Age<br />

differences in performance <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> color-word test. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

Pers<strong>on</strong>ality, 31, 534-558.<br />

Ratclitf, R. (1978). A <strong>the</strong>ory <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> memory retrieval. Psychological Re-<br />

view, 85, 59-108.<br />

Ray, C. (1974). The manipulati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> color resp<strong>on</strong>se times in a color-<br />

word interference task. Percepti<strong>on</strong> and Psychophysics, 16, 101-104.<br />

Rayner, K., & Posnansky, C. J. (1978). Stages <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing in word<br />

identificati<strong>on</strong>. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psychology: General, 107,<br />

64-80.<br />

Rayner, K., & Springer, C. J. (1986). Graphemic and semantic similar-<br />

ity effects in <strong>the</strong> picture-word interference task. British Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

Psychology, 77, 207-222.<br />

Redding, G. M., & Gerjets, D. A. (1977). <strong>Stroop</strong> effect: Interference and<br />

facilitati<strong>on</strong> with verbal and manual resp<strong>on</strong>ses. Perceptual and Motor<br />

Skills, 45, 11-17.<br />

Regan, J. E. (1979). Automatic processing. (Doctoral dissertati<strong>on</strong>, Uni-<br />

versity <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> California, Berkeley, 1977). Dissertati<strong>on</strong> Abstracts Interna-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong>al 39, 1018-B.<br />

Regan, J. E. (1978). Involuntary automatic processing in color-naming<br />

tasks. Percepti<strong>on</strong> and Psychophysics, 24, 130-136.<br />

Regan, J. E. (1981). Automaticity and learning: <strong>Effect</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> familiarity <strong>on</strong><br />

naming letters. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psychology: Human Percep-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong> and Performance, 7, 180-195.<br />

Reiner, M. B., & Morris<strong>on</strong>, E J. (1983). Is semantic interference really<br />

automatic? Bulletin <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> Psych<strong>on</strong>omic Society, 21, 27 !-274.<br />

Reisberg, D., Bar<strong>on</strong>, J., & Kemler, D. G. (1980). Overcoming <strong>Stroop</strong><br />

interference: The effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> practice <strong>on</strong> distractor potency. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

Experimental Psychology: Human Percepti<strong>on</strong> and Performance, 6,<br />

140-150.<br />

Ridley, D. R. 0980). Color-word c<strong>on</strong>notative inc<strong>on</strong>gruity: A replica-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong>. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 50, 1309-1310.<br />

Ridley, D. R., Johns<strong>on</strong>, D. E., & Braisted, E D. (1978). The color-word<br />

c<strong>on</strong>notative inc<strong>on</strong>gruity effect. PerceptualandMotor Skills, 46, 939-<br />

946.<br />

Rieck, A. M., & Coates, G. D. 0977, November). Irrelevant informati<strong>on</strong><br />

effects in <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> color-word test. Paper presented at <strong>the</strong> annual<br />

meeting <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> Psych<strong>on</strong>omic Society, Washingt<strong>on</strong>, DC.<br />

Roe, W. T., Wils<strong>on</strong>cr<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>t, W. E., & Grittiths, R. S. (1980). <strong>Effect</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> motor<br />

and verbal practice <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> task. Perceptual and Motor Skills,<br />

50, 647-650.<br />

Rosinski, R. R. (1977). Picture-word interference is semantically<br />

based. Child Development, 48, 643-647.<br />

Rosinski, R. R., Golink<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>f, R. M., & Kukish, K. S. (1975). Automatic<br />

semantic processing in a picture-word interference task. ChildDevel-<br />

opment, 46, 247-253.<br />

Rumelhart, D. E., Hint<strong>on</strong>, (3. E., & McClelland, J. L. (1986). A general<br />

framework for parallel distributed processing. In D. E. Rumelhart,<br />

J. L. McClelland, & <strong>the</strong> PDP <str<strong>on</strong>g>Research</str<strong>on</strong>g> Group (Eds.), Paralleldistrib-<br />

uted processing: Explorati<strong>on</strong>s in <strong>the</strong> microstructure <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> cogniti<strong>on</strong>, Iiol. 1<br />

(pp. 45-76). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.<br />

Santos, J. E, & M<strong>on</strong>tgomery, J. R. (1962). Stability <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> performance <strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> color-word test. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 15, 397-398.<br />

Sarmanay, I. (1977). Different performance in <strong>Stroop</strong>'s Interference<br />

Test from <strong>the</strong> aspect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> pers<strong>on</strong>ality and sex. Studia Psychologica, 19,<br />

60-67. (From Psychological Abstracts, 1978, 59, Abstract No. 2543)<br />

Saunders, D. R. 0980). Definiti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>Stroop</strong> interference in volunteers<br />

and n<strong>on</strong>-volunteers. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 51, 343-354.<br />

Scheibe, K., Shaver, E R., & Carrier, S. C. (1967). Color associati<strong>on</strong><br />

values and resp<strong>on</strong>se interference <strong>on</strong> variants <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> test. Acta<br />

Psychologica, 26, 286-295.<br />

Schiller, E H. (1966). Developmental s(udy <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> color-word interference.<br />

Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psychology, 72, 105-108.<br />

Schmit, V., & Davis, R. (1974). The role <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> hemispheric specializati<strong>on</strong><br />

in <strong>the</strong> analysis <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>Stroop</strong> stimuli. Acta Psychologica, 38, 149-158.<br />

Schneider, W., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1977). C<strong>on</strong>trolled and automatic hu-<br />

man informati<strong>on</strong> processing: I. Detecti<strong>on</strong>, search, and attenti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Psychological <strong>Review</strong>, 84, 1-66.<br />

Schubo, W., & Hentschel, U. (1977). Reliability and validity <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> serial<br />

Color-Word Test: Fur<strong>the</strong>r results. Psychological <str<strong>on</strong>g>Research</str<strong>on</strong>g> Bulletin,<br />

Lund University, 17. (From Psychological Abstracts, 1979, 61, Ab-<br />

stract No. 5169)<br />

Schubo, W., & Hentschel, U. (1978). Improved reliability estimates for<br />

<strong>the</strong> serial Color-Word Test. Scandinavian Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Psychology, 19,<br />

91-95.<br />

Schulz, T (1979). Comp<strong>on</strong>ents <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> reacti<strong>on</strong> time <strong>Stroop</strong>-task. Psy-<br />

chological <str<strong>on</strong>g>Research</str<strong>on</strong>g>, 40, 377-395.<br />

Schweikert, R. (1978). A critical path generalizati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> additive<br />

factor method: <strong>An</strong>alysis <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a <strong>Stroop</strong> task. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Ma<strong>the</strong>matical<br />

Psychology, 18, 105-139.<br />

Schweikert, R. (1983). Latent network <strong>the</strong>ory: Scheduling <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processes<br />

in sentence verificati<strong>on</strong> and <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> effect. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimen-<br />

tal Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cogniti<strong>on</strong>, 9, 353-383.<br />

Scott, D. E, Hotfman, H. J., & Bickford, R. G. (1967). Changes in<br />

summated visual potentials (lambda waves) during mental tasks us-<br />

ing <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> test. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 25, 993-996.<br />

Severance, L. J., & Dyer, E N. (1973). Failure o fsubliminal word presen-<br />

tati<strong>on</strong>s to generate interference to color naming. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experi-<br />

mental Psychology, 101, 186-189.<br />

Seymour, E H. (1973). <strong>Stroop</strong> interference in naming and verifying<br />

spatial locati<strong>on</strong>s. Percepti<strong>on</strong> and Psychophysics, 14, 95-100.<br />

Seymour, E H. (1974). <strong>Stroop</strong> interference with resp<strong>on</strong>se, comparis<strong>on</strong>,<br />

and encoding stages in a sentence-picture comparis<strong>on</strong> task. Memory<br />

and Cogniti<strong>on</strong>, 2, 19-26.<br />

Seymour, E H. (1977). C<strong>on</strong>ceptual encoding and locus <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong><br />

effect. Quarterly Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psychology, 29, 245-265.<br />

Shaffer, W. O., & LaBerge, D. (i 979). Automatic semantic processing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

unattended words. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior,<br />

18, 413-426.<br />

Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, W. (1977). C<strong>on</strong>trolled and automatic hu-<br />

man informati<strong>on</strong> processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic at-<br />

tending and a general <strong>the</strong>ory. Psychological <strong>Review</strong>, 84, 127-190.<br />

Shor, R. E. (1970). The processing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>ceptual informati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> spatial<br />

directi<strong>on</strong>s from pictorial and linguistic symbols. Acta Psychologica,<br />

32, 346-365.<br />

Shor, R. E. (1971). Symbol processing speed differences and symbol<br />

interference effects in a variety <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>cept domains. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Gen-<br />

eral Psychology, 85, 187-205.<br />

Shor, R. E. (1975). <strong>An</strong> auditory analog <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> test. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

General Psychology, 93, 281-288.<br />

Shor, R. E., Hatch, R. E, Huds<strong>on</strong>, L. J., Landrigan, D. T., & Shaffer,<br />

H. J. (1972). <strong>Effect</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> practice <strong>on</strong> a <strong>Stroop</strong>-like spatial directi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

task. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psychology, 94, 168-172.<br />

Sichel, J. L., & Chandler, K. A. (1969). The color-word interference test:<br />

The effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> varied color-word combinati<strong>on</strong>s up<strong>on</strong> verbal resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

latency. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Psychology, 72, 219-231.<br />

Silverstein, A. B., & Franken, R. E. (I 965). Performance <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> mentally<br />

retarded <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> Color-Word Test. Perceptualand Motor Skills,<br />

21, 618.<br />

Sim<strong>on</strong>, J. R., & Sudalaimuthu, E (1979). <strong>Effect</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> S-R mapping and<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se modality <strong>on</strong> performance in a <strong>Stroop</strong> task. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Ex-<br />

perimental Psychology: Human Percepti<strong>on</strong> and Performance, 5, 176-<br />

187.<br />

Singer, M. H., Lappin, J. S., & Moore, L. E (1975). The interference <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

various word parts <strong>on</strong> color naming in <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> test. Percepti<strong>on</strong><br />

and Psychophysics, 18, 191 - 193.<br />

Sjt~berg, L. (1969). On serial scoring <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> color word test. Acta Psy-<br />

chologica, 29, 150-162.


Sjtiberg, L. (I 974). Psychometric properties <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> serial colour word<br />

test. Scandinavian Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Psychology, 15, 15-20.<br />

Sladekova, L., & Daniel, J. (1981). Differences in performance in<br />

<strong>Stroop</strong>'s test from <strong>the</strong> aspect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> sex and age. Studia Psychologica, 23,<br />

145-149.<br />

Smekal, V., & Dvoracek, V. (1977). Relati<strong>on</strong>ship <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> perceptual interfer-<br />

ence and pers<strong>on</strong>ality variables in 7th-grade students. Psychologia a<br />

Patopsychologia Dietata, 12, 47-54. (From Psychological Abstracts,<br />

1979, 62, Abstract No. 10658)<br />

Smith, G. J. W. (1959). Comparis<strong>on</strong>s between adaptive patterns in two<br />

serial experiments. Acta Psychologica, 16, 302-315.<br />

Smith, G. J. W (1970). Change in degree <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interference in a <strong>Stroop</strong>-like<br />

test (SWT) when <strong>the</strong> inc<strong>on</strong>gruent combinati<strong>on</strong>s are mixed with c<strong>on</strong>-<br />

gruent <strong>on</strong>es. Psychologische Forschung, 34, 95-96.<br />

Smith, G. J. W., & Borg, G. A. V. (1964). The problem <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>retesting in <strong>the</strong><br />

serial color-word test. Psychological <str<strong>on</strong>g>Research</str<strong>on</strong>g> Bulletin, Lund Univer-<br />

sity, 4, 1-8.<br />

Smith, G. J. W., & Klein, G. S. (1953). Cognitive c<strong>on</strong>trols in serial behav-<br />

ior patterns. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Pers<strong>on</strong>ality, 22, 188-213.<br />

Smith, G. J. W., & Nyman, G. E. (1974). The validity <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> serial color-<br />

word test: A reply to Lennart Sj6berg. Scandinavian Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Psy-<br />

chology, 15, 238-240.<br />

Smith, M. C., & Kirsner, K. (1982). Language and orthography as irrele-<br />

vant features in colour-word and picture-word <strong>Stroop</strong> interference.<br />

Quarterly Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental<br />

Psychology, 34.4, 153-170.<br />

Smith, M. C., & Magee, L. E. (1980). Tracing <strong>the</strong> time course <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> picture-<br />

word processing. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psychology: General 109,<br />

373-392.<br />

Solso, R. L. (1971 ). Meaningfulness <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> colors. Psych<strong>on</strong>omic Science, 23,<br />

301-303.<br />

Stanovich, K. E., & Pachella, R. G. (1977). Encoding, stimulus-re-<br />

sp<strong>on</strong>se compatibility, and stages<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>processing. Journal<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>Experimen-<br />

tal Psychology." Human Percepti<strong>on</strong> and Performance, 3, 411-421.<br />

Stifling, N. (1979). <strong>Stroop</strong> interference: <strong>An</strong> input and an output phe-<br />

nomen<strong>on</strong>. Quarterly Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psychology, 31, 121-<br />

132.<br />

Stifling, N., & Coi<strong>the</strong>art, M. (1977). <strong>Stroop</strong> interference in a letter nam-<br />

ing task. Bulletin <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> Psych<strong>on</strong>omic Society, 10, 31-34.<br />

<strong>Stroop</strong>, J. R. (1935a). The basis <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Lig<strong>on</strong>'s <strong>the</strong>ory. American Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

Psychology, 47, 499-504.<br />

<strong>Stroop</strong>, J. R. (1935b). Studies <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interference in serial verbal reacti<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psychology, 18, 643-662.<br />

<strong>Stroop</strong>, J. R. (1938). Factors affecting speed in serial verbal reacti<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Psychological M<strong>on</strong>ographs, 50, 38-48.<br />

Tanenhaus, M. K., Flanigan, H. P., & Seidenberg, M. S. (i 980). Ortho-<br />

graphic and ph<strong>on</strong>ological activati<strong>on</strong> in auditory and visual word<br />

recogniti<strong>on</strong>. Memory and Cogniti<strong>on</strong>, 8, 513-520.<br />

Taylor, A., & Clive, P. B. (1983). Two forms <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> test. Percep-<br />

tual and Motor Skills, 57, 879-882.<br />

Taylor, D, A. (1977). Time course <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>text effects. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experi-<br />

mental Psychology: General, 106, 404-426.<br />

Tecce, J. J., & Dimartino, M. (1965). <strong>Effect</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> heightened drive (shock)<br />

<strong>on</strong> performance in a tachistoscopic color-word interference task.<br />

Psychological Reports, 16, 93-94.<br />

Tecce, J. J., & Happ, S. J. (1964). <strong>Effect</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> shock-arousal <strong>on</strong> a card-sort-<br />

ing test <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> color-word interference. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 19,<br />

905-906.<br />

Telford, C. W. (1930). Differences in resp<strong>on</strong>ses to colors and to <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

names: Some racial comparis<strong>on</strong>s. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Genetic Psychology, 37,<br />

151-159.<br />

Thomas, J. K. (1977). <strong>Stroop</strong> interference with word or hue pre-expo-<br />

sure (Doctoral dissertati<strong>on</strong>, University <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Michigan, 1977). Disserta-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong> Abstracts Internati<strong>on</strong>al 38, ! 450-B.<br />

Thurst<strong>on</strong>e, L. L. (1944). A factorial study <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> percepti<strong>on</strong>. Chicago: Uni-<br />

versity <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Chicago Press.<br />

THE STROOP EFFECT 201<br />

Tipper, S. E (1985). The negative priming effect: Inhibitory priming by<br />

ignored objects. Quarterly Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psychology, 37A,<br />

571-590.<br />

Tipper, S. P., & Cranst<strong>on</strong>, M. (1985). Selective attenti<strong>on</strong> and priming:<br />

Inhibitory and facilitatory effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> ignored primes. Quarterly Jour-<br />

nal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psychology, 37.4, 591-611.<br />

Toma, R. J., & Tsao, Y.-C. (1985). Interference effects in <strong>the</strong> picture-<br />

word <strong>Stroop</strong> task. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 61, 223-228.<br />

Treisman, A. M. (1969). Strategies and models <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> selective attenti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Psychological <strong>Review</strong>, 76, 282-299.<br />

Treisman, A. M., & Fearnley, S. (1969). The <strong>Stroop</strong> test: Selective atten-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong> to colours and words. Nature, 222, 437--439.<br />

Tsao, Y.-C., Feustel, T., & Soseos, C. (1979). <strong>Stroop</strong> interference in <strong>the</strong><br />

left and right visual fields. Brain and Language, 8, 367-371.<br />

Tsao, Y.-C., Wu, M.-E, & Feustel, T. (1981). <strong>Stroop</strong> interference: Hemi-<br />

spheric difference in Chinese speakers. Brain and Language, 13,<br />

372-378.<br />

Tschopp, M., & Jorswieck, E. (1976). Comparis<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> shifting between<br />

groups <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> mentally retarded children, sec<strong>on</strong>dary school children<br />

and seniors. Praxis der Kinderpsychologie und Kinderpsychiatrie, 25,<br />

22-24. (From Psychological Abstracts, 1981, 65, Abstract No. 1524)<br />

Uechi, Y. (1972). Cognitive interference and intelligence: Reexamina-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> measures <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>SCWT, Japanese Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Educati<strong>on</strong>al Psy-<br />

chology, 20, 92-100. (From Psychological Abstracts, 1974, 52, Ab-<br />

stract No. 12745)<br />

Uhlman, E W (1962). Test <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> color recogniti<strong>on</strong>. Detroit, MI: The Detroit<br />

Edis<strong>on</strong> Co.<br />

U leman, J. S., & Reeves, J. (1971). A reversal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> interference<br />

effect, through scanning. Percepti<strong>on</strong> and Psychophysics, 9, 293-295.<br />

Underwood, G. (1976). Semantic interference from unattended<br />

printed words. British Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Psychology, 67, 327-338.<br />

Underwood, G,, & Briggs, P. (1984). The development <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> word recogni-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong> processes. British Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Psychology, 75, 243-255.<br />

Underwood, G., Briggs, P., & Underwood, J. D. (1984). Cognitive pro-<br />

cesses in children's reading attainment. Human Learning: Journal<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

Practical <str<strong>on</strong>g>Research</str<strong>on</strong>g> and Applicati<strong>on</strong>s, 3, 119-128,<br />

Vaid, J., & Lambert, W E. (1979). Differential cerebral involvement in<br />

<strong>the</strong> cognitive functi<strong>on</strong>ing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> bilinguals. Brain and Language, 8, 92-<br />

110.<br />

Virzi, R. A., & Egeth, H. E. (1984). Is meaning implicated in illusory<br />

c<strong>on</strong>juncti<strong>on</strong>s? Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psychology: Human Percep-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong> and Performance, 10, 573-580.<br />

Virzi, R. A., & Egeth, H. E. (1985). Toward a translati<strong>on</strong>al model <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

<strong>Stroop</strong> interference. Memory and Cogniti<strong>on</strong>, 13, 304-319.<br />

Warren, L. R., & Marsh, G. R. (1978). Hemispheric asymmetry in <strong>the</strong><br />

processing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>Stroop</strong> stimuli. Bulletin <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> Psych<strong>on</strong>omic Society, 12,<br />

214-216.<br />

Warren, L. R., & Marsh, G. R. (1979). Changes in event related poten-<br />

tials during processing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>Stroop</strong> stimuli. Internati<strong>on</strong>al Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

Neuroscience, 9, 217-223.<br />

Warren, R. E. (1972). Stimulus encoding and memory. Journal<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>Exper-<br />

imental Psychology, 94, 90-100.<br />

Warren, R. E. (1974). Associati<strong>on</strong>, directi<strong>on</strong>ality, and stimulus encod-<br />

ing. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psychology, 102, 151-158.<br />

Warren, R. E., & Lasher, M. D. (1974). Interference in a typeface vari-<br />

ant <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> test. Percepti<strong>on</strong> and Psychophysics, 15, 128-130.<br />

Watts, E N., McKenna, E T., Sharrock, R., & Trezise, L. (1986). Colour-<br />

naming <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> phobia-related words. British Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Psychology, 77,<br />

97-108.<br />

West, R. E, & Stanovich, K. E. (1978). Automatic c<strong>on</strong>textual facilita-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong> in readers <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> three ages. Child Development, 49, 717-727.<br />

Westberry, R. L. (1984). The nature <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>attenti<strong>on</strong>al c<strong>on</strong>trol as a pers<strong>on</strong>al-<br />

ity dimensi<strong>on</strong> (Doctoral dissertati<strong>on</strong>, University <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> South Florida,<br />

Tampa, 1983). Dissertati<strong>on</strong> Abstracts Internati<strong>on</strong>al 45, 1043-B.


202 COLIN M. MACLEOD<br />

Wheeler, D. D. (1977). Locus <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interference <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> test. Percep-<br />

tual and Motor Skills, 45, 263-266.<br />

White, B. W (1969). Interference in identifying attributes and attribute<br />

names. Percepti<strong>on</strong> and Psychophysics, 6, 166-168.<br />

White, U C. (1978). Interference pr<strong>on</strong>eness and <strong>the</strong> ability to shift at-<br />

tenti<strong>on</strong> in old age (Doctoral dissertati<strong>on</strong>, University <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Notre Dame,<br />

1978). Dissertati<strong>on</strong> Abstracts Internati<strong>on</strong>al, 39, 2549-B.<br />

Whitney, P. (1986). Processing category terms in c<strong>on</strong>text: Instantia-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong>s as inferences. Memory and Cogniti<strong>on</strong>, 14, 39-48.<br />

Whitney, E, & Kellas, G. (1984). Processing category terms in c<strong>on</strong>text:<br />

Instantiati<strong>on</strong> and <strong>the</strong> structure <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> semantic categories. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cogniti<strong>on</strong>, 1 O, 95-<br />

103.<br />

Whitney, P., McKay, T., Kellas, G., & Emers<strong>on</strong>, W. A., Jr. (1985). Seman-<br />

tic activati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> noun c<strong>on</strong>cepts in c<strong>on</strong>text. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental<br />

Psychology: Learning, Memo~ and Cogniti<strong>on</strong>, 11, 126-135.<br />

Wilder, L. (1969). A developmental viewpoint c<strong>on</strong>cerning <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong><br />

color-word test and verbal interference. Speech M<strong>on</strong>ographs, 36,<br />

114-117.<br />

Appendix A<br />

Organizati<strong>on</strong>al Structure <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Review</strong><br />

Williams, E. (1977). The effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> amount o finformati<strong>on</strong> in <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong><br />

color word test. Percepti<strong>on</strong> and Psychophysics, 22, 463-470.<br />

Windes, J. D. (1968). Reacti<strong>on</strong> time for numerical coding and naming<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> numerals. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Experimental Psychology, 78, 318-322.<br />

Wise, L. A., Sutt<strong>on</strong>, J. A., & Gibb<strong>on</strong>s, E D. (1975). Decrement in <strong>Stroop</strong><br />

interference time with age. Perceptualand Motor Skills, 41,149-150.<br />

Wolitzky, D. L., H<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>er, R., & Shapiro, R. (1972). Cognitive c<strong>on</strong>trols and<br />

mental retardati<strong>on</strong>. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Abnormal Psychology, 79, 296-302.<br />

Woodworth, R. S., & Wells, E L. (1911). Associati<strong>on</strong> tests. Psychologi-<br />

cal <strong>Review</strong> M<strong>on</strong>ograph Supplements, 13(No. 57).<br />

Zajano, M. J., & Gorman, A. (1986). <strong>Stroop</strong> interference as a functi<strong>on</strong><br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> percentage <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>gruent items. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 63,<br />

1087-1096.<br />

Zajano, M. J., Hoyceanyls, E. M., & Ouellette, J. E (1981). A c<strong>on</strong>found<br />

in <strong>the</strong> standard c<strong>on</strong>trol c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> experiment. Percep-<br />

tual and Motor Skills, 53, 835-841.<br />

Zakay, D., & Glicksohn, J. (1985). Stimulus c<strong>on</strong>gruity and S -R compati-<br />

bility as determinants <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interference in a <strong>Stroop</strong>-like task. Canadian<br />

Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Psychology, 39, 414--423.<br />

Topic Page Topic Page<br />

Variati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> procedure<br />

The <strong>Stroop</strong> Color-Word Interference Test<br />

The individual stimulus versi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong><br />

color-word task<br />

Sorting and matching versi<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> color-<br />

word task<br />

The picture-word interference task<br />

Auditory analogs <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> task<br />

O<strong>the</strong>r analogs <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> task<br />

Manipulati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> informati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> critical trials<br />

Hue variati<strong>on</strong><br />

Acoustic variati<strong>on</strong><br />

Semantic variati<strong>on</strong><br />

Semantic variati<strong>on</strong> and <strong>the</strong> irrelevant word<br />

Semantic variati<strong>on</strong> induced by priming<br />

C<strong>on</strong>gruency effects<br />

Integrati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> two dimensi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

Experiment-wide manipulati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

informati<strong>on</strong><br />

Probability <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> various trial types<br />

Stimulus set size<br />

165<br />

165<br />

166<br />

166<br />

167<br />

168<br />

168<br />

170<br />

170<br />

170<br />

172<br />

172<br />

173<br />

174<br />

175<br />

176<br />

176<br />

177<br />

Trial sequence 177<br />

Pretrial cues 178<br />

Stimulus <strong>on</strong>set ~ynchr<strong>on</strong>y 179<br />

The reverse <strong>Stroop</strong> effect 180<br />

Practice 181<br />

Resp<strong>on</strong>se-related manipulati<strong>on</strong>s 182<br />

Order <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> resp<strong>on</strong>se 182<br />

Resp<strong>on</strong>se modality: oral versus manual 182<br />

Resp<strong>on</strong>se set size and compositi<strong>on</strong> 183<br />

Individual differences 184<br />

Sex differences 184<br />

~ge differences 184<br />

Hemispheric differences 185<br />

Language differences: <strong>the</strong> case <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong><br />

bilingual 186<br />

Theoretical accounts <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> effect 187<br />

Relative speed <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> processing 187<br />

Automaticity 188<br />

Perceptual encoding 190<br />

Parallel models: accruing evidence toward a<br />

decisi<strong>on</strong> 191<br />

A parallel distributed processing model 192


1. The <strong>Stroop</strong> effect is observed with lists <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> stimuli, with single stim-<br />

uli, and with many variati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> <strong>the</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>se required. Similar<br />

data patterns are evident in numerous <strong>Stroop</strong> analogs, such as <strong>the</strong><br />

picture-word task.<br />

2. Both orthographic and particularly acoustic/articulatory relati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

between <strong>the</strong> irrelevant word (or part <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> word) and <strong>the</strong> to-be-<br />

named ink color c<strong>on</strong>tribute to <strong>the</strong> interference.<br />

3. Compared with naming <strong>the</strong> ink color al<strong>on</strong>e, irrelevant verbal stim-<br />

uli that are unrelated to <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>cept <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> color interfere <strong>on</strong>ly mini-<br />

mally with color naming. However, as <strong>the</strong> word's semantic associa-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong> to <strong>the</strong> c<strong>on</strong>cept <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> color increases, so does its power to interfere.<br />

4. A color-unrelated word can be made to cause greater interference<br />

(or facilitati<strong>on</strong>, or both) with color naming if its meaning is acti-<br />

vated by a related word or phrase shortly before <strong>the</strong> color-naming<br />

trial.<br />

5. C<strong>on</strong>gruence between <strong>the</strong> irrelevant word and <strong>the</strong> to-be-named ink<br />

color <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>ten produces facilitati<strong>on</strong>. However, this facilitati<strong>on</strong> is much<br />

less than <strong>the</strong> corresp<strong>on</strong>ding interference in <strong>the</strong> inc<strong>on</strong>gruent c<strong>on</strong>di-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong>, and <strong>the</strong> choice <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>trol c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> may be crucial.<br />

6. If <strong>the</strong> to-be-named color and <strong>the</strong> to-be-ignored word are presented<br />

in separate spatial locati<strong>on</strong>s, interference will be reduced (but not<br />

eliminated) relative to <strong>the</strong> standard, integrated versi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> task.<br />

Locati<strong>on</strong>al uncertainty makes an important c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong> in n<strong>on</strong>-<br />

integrated situati<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

7. The presence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>gruent trials am<strong>on</strong>g <strong>the</strong> inc<strong>on</strong>gruent and c<strong>on</strong>-<br />

trol trials will tend to invoke <strong>the</strong> tactic <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> splitting attenti<strong>on</strong> over <strong>the</strong><br />

two dimensi<strong>on</strong>s, <strong>the</strong>reby increasing interference <strong>on</strong> inc<strong>on</strong>gruent<br />

trials.<br />

8. When <strong>the</strong> irrelevant word <strong>on</strong> trial n - l is <strong>the</strong> name <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> target ink<br />

color <strong>on</strong> trial n, interference with color naming will be enhanced<br />

temporarily; when <strong>the</strong> ink color <strong>on</strong> trial n - l matches <strong>the</strong> word <strong>on</strong><br />

trial n, <strong>the</strong>re will be some facilitati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> color naming <strong>on</strong> trial n. If<br />

<strong>the</strong> word <strong>on</strong> trial n - 1 is repeated <strong>on</strong> trial n, <strong>the</strong>n <strong>the</strong> word is<br />

already suppressed and will cause less interference in naming a<br />

different ink color <strong>on</strong> trial n.<br />

9. Advance cues c<strong>on</strong>veying informati<strong>on</strong> about <strong>the</strong> upcoming <strong>Stroop</strong><br />

trial can be used to establish processing strategies that improve<br />

performance if <strong>the</strong>se cues are above <strong>the</strong> level <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> subjective aware-<br />

ness and ifa very small set <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> cues is used c<strong>on</strong>sistently.<br />

THE STROOP EFFECT<br />

Appendix B<br />

Eighteen Major Empirical Results That Must be Explained<br />

by <strong>An</strong>y Successful Account <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Stroop</strong> <strong>Effect</strong><br />

203<br />

10. When <strong>the</strong> color (or picture) is to be named, maximal impact <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a<br />

c<strong>on</strong>gruent or inc<strong>on</strong>gruent word will be observed when <strong>the</strong> two<br />

dimensi<strong>on</strong>s begin within 100 ms <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> each o<strong>the</strong>r. Facilitati<strong>on</strong> may<br />

extend to l<strong>on</strong>ger SOAs than interference when <strong>the</strong> word comes<br />

first. Manipulating SOA has virtually no impact <strong>on</strong> word reading<br />

unless a very high proporti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>gruent trials biases use <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong><br />

color to initiate resp<strong>on</strong>se producti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

11. A reverse <strong>Stroop</strong> effect (i.e., interference with word reading caused<br />

by an incompatible, irrelevant ink color) appears to be possible,<br />

but this effect is not simply a c<strong>on</strong>sequence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> relative speeds <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

processing each dimensi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

12. Degree <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>practice in processing each <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><strong>the</strong> dimensi<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a multi-<br />

dimensi<strong>on</strong>al stimulus is very influential in determining <strong>the</strong> extent<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> interference from <strong>on</strong>e dimensi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> ano<strong>the</strong>r. The greater <strong>the</strong><br />

practice in processing a dimensi<strong>on</strong>, <strong>the</strong> more capable that dimen-<br />

si<strong>on</strong> seems <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> influencing <strong>the</strong> processing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> ano<strong>the</strong>r dimensi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

13. Although still significant, interference (but perhaps not facilita-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong>) is reduced when resp<strong>on</strong>se modality is switched from oral to<br />

manual. Stimulus-resp<strong>on</strong>se compatibility matters; if <strong>the</strong> normal<br />

processing <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> irrelevant dimensi<strong>on</strong> leads to a resp<strong>on</strong>se in <strong>the</strong><br />

mode designated for <strong>the</strong> relevant dimensi<strong>on</strong>, interference is likely<br />

to be heightened.<br />

14. When <strong>the</strong> irrelevant dimensi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a set <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> stimuli includes names<br />

that are eligible resp<strong>on</strong>ses for <strong>the</strong> relevant dimensi<strong>on</strong>, more interfer-<br />

ence results than when <strong>the</strong> sets are n<strong>on</strong>overlapping. Although varia-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong>s in resp<strong>on</strong>se set size might be expected to affect interference,<br />

existing results are unclear.<br />

15. There are no sex differences in <strong>Stroop</strong> interference at any age.<br />

16. Interference begins early in <strong>the</strong> school years, rising to its highest<br />

level around Grades 2 to 3 as reading skill develops. With c<strong>on</strong>tin-<br />

ued development <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reading, interference declines through <strong>the</strong><br />

adult years until approximately age 60, at which point it begins to<br />

increase again.<br />

17. The left hemisphere generally shows more interference than <strong>the</strong><br />

right.<br />

18. Interference between <strong>the</strong> two languages <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a bilingual, although<br />

not as great as that within ei<strong>the</strong>r <strong>on</strong>e <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>the</strong> languages, is very ro-<br />

bust: Between language interference typically is about 75% <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

within-language interference. Fur<strong>the</strong>rmore, a dominant language<br />

has more potential for interfering than does a n<strong>on</strong>dominant <strong>on</strong>e.<br />

Received August 18, 1989<br />

Revisi<strong>on</strong> received May 29, 1990<br />

Accepted June 8, 1990 •

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!