Download (23MB) - University of Salford Institutional Repository

Download (23MB) - University of Salford Institutional Repository Download (23MB) - University of Salford Institutional Repository

usir.salford.ac.uk
from usir.salford.ac.uk More from this publisher
23.03.2013 Views

as face and politeness. Although topic is widely recognised as a central organising feature in conversational interaction (Brown and Yule 1983), several scholars have noted that defining, identifying, and labelling topic(s) in naturally occurring conversation is far from clear cut. (Atkinson and Heritage 1984) In fact, several scholars have come to regard attempts to formally'pin down' topic as perhaps an impossible task (Brown and Yule 1983). There is a considerable body of work which goes some way to aiding an identification of topic in discourse, focusing on things such as topic initiators or elicitors (e. g. Schegloff and Sacks 1973; Wilson 1989), topic transitions such as switching, drifting, or shading from one topic to another (e. g. Drew and Holt 1998; Jefferson 1993; Crow 1983), and techniques for topic closings (e. g. Schegloff and Sacks 1973). In terms of what the 'content' of the topic actually is, a range of work has attempted to identify the linguistic basis for deriving topic, ranging from studies of the grammatical constituents of noun phrases, through propositional content of contiguous utterances, to more global notions of 'aboutness'(e. g. Brown and Yule 1983, Bublitz 1988; Maynard 1980, Tannen 1984). 1 will not attempt to enter into the debate about what topic is, or should be as a discourse notion here, but rather should mention briefly how the notion of topic afforded me some analytical purchase for my initial analysis of sociable episodes. First, there were practical considerations. The total corpus of conversational data on which this study is based exceeds 70 hours (see table 4.2). It would not have been possible for me as a single researcher to transcribe all this data. Even with the memo-scriber (see table 4.1), the sample I selected from this total data for closer analysis required many hours to transcribe'. More generally, I would have found it impossible to maintain an overview of all the data, or make sense of it in any systematic 'sociological' way. I thus thought it more prudent to concentrate on one aspect of discourse, rather than spread myself thin as it were, over the whole data set'. Second, there were empirical considerations. As I noted earlier (see Chapter 2) topic talk had been demonstrated to be one of the salient ways in which German and English conversational style differed, in terms of both the general orientation to topic development, and the positive social values oriented to and images of selfhood 126

claimed on the part of the participants (Byrnes 1986, Straehle 1997, Watts 1989). Thus, focusing on topic would give some access to the conversational reality that I was interested in. Further, focusing on episodes of topic talk from both speech communities would, I hope, provide in effect a standardised procedure for eliciting a 'sample frame'. Topic would seem then to add both validity (topic can be taken as a valid indicator of conversational style) and reliability (topic occurs ubiquitously throughout talk and may therefore be drawn upon consistently across studies addressing other cultures) to the study as a whole. Third, there were analytical considerations. As demonstrated by scholars such as Tannen (1984) topic can provide a basis for deriving an initial unit of analysis of conversational activity, particularly for the close inspection of the type and manner of self-presentation, which, as I suggested in Chapter 3, is a primary concern here. More specifically, the way topic is drawn upon and managed has been shown to be informed by face concerns of interlocutors. The classic example of face influenced topic choice and development, cited by both Goffman (1967) and Brown and Levinson (1987) is that of the idea of 'safe topics' (e. g. the weather). Potentially face-threatening topics are usually avoided, or, handled in their development in a way that will avoid threatening any of the faces of the participants. Aside from the risk to face that guide conversational ists in their choice and development of topic, scholars such as Tannen have also identified topic as a site for presentation of self. Thus, conversational topic appeared to be an ideal site to examine the interplay of both presentational practices and face-concerns. I outlined in Chapter 1 that facework could be conceived of as occurring within episodes. In Chapter 31 further drew on the notion of episodes within sociable gatherings to arrive at the term 'sociable episodes'. The concept of episode has been employed frequently in studies addressing ongoing discourse (e. g. Katriel 1986; Malone 1997; Tannen 1984; Watson and Potter 1962) as well as those more specifically concerned with facework (e. g. Penman 1990; Wood and Kroger 1991). The term however is quite a fuzzy one, and implies an objective and bounded period of interaction which is normally ill defined in studies applying the term. I will not attempt a formal definition here beyond treating an episode as a period of interaction where participants are 'doing' some common conversational topic together whereby they share a 127

as face and politeness. Although topic is widely recognised as a central<br />

organising feature in conversational interaction (Brown and Yule 1983), several<br />

scholars have noted that defining, identifying, and labelling topic(s) in naturally<br />

occurring conversation is far from clear cut. (Atkinson and Heritage 1984) In<br />

fact, several scholars have come to regard attempts to formally'pin down' topic<br />

as perhaps an impossible task (Brown and Yule 1983).<br />

There is a considerable body <strong>of</strong> work which goes some way to aiding an<br />

identification <strong>of</strong> topic in discourse, focusing on things such as topic initiators or<br />

elicitors (e. g. Schegl<strong>of</strong>f and Sacks 1973; Wilson 1989), topic transitions such as<br />

switching, drifting, or shading from one topic to another (e. g. Drew and Holt<br />

1998; Jefferson 1993; Crow 1983), and techniques for topic closings (e. g.<br />

Schegl<strong>of</strong>f and Sacks 1973). In terms <strong>of</strong> what the 'content' <strong>of</strong> the topic actually is,<br />

a range <strong>of</strong> work has attempted to identify the linguistic basis for deriving topic,<br />

ranging from studies <strong>of</strong> the grammatical constituents <strong>of</strong> noun phrases, through<br />

propositional content <strong>of</strong> contiguous utterances, to more global notions <strong>of</strong><br />

'aboutness'(e. g. Brown and Yule 1983, Bublitz 1988; Maynard 1980, Tannen<br />

1984). 1 will not attempt to enter into the debate about what topic is, or should<br />

be as a discourse notion here, but rather should mention briefly how the notion<br />

<strong>of</strong> topic afforded me some analytical purchase for my initial analysis <strong>of</strong> sociable<br />

episodes.<br />

First, there were practical considerations. The total corpus <strong>of</strong><br />

conversational data on which this study is based exceeds 70 hours (see table<br />

4.2). It would not have been possible for me as a single researcher to transcribe<br />

all this data. Even with the memo-scriber (see table 4.1), the sample I selected<br />

from this total data for closer analysis required many hours to transcribe'. More<br />

generally, I would have found it impossible to maintain an overview <strong>of</strong> all the<br />

data, or make sense <strong>of</strong> it in any systematic 'sociological' way. I thus thought it<br />

more prudent to concentrate on one aspect <strong>of</strong> discourse, rather than spread<br />

myself thin as it were, over the whole data set'. Second, there were empirical<br />

considerations. As I noted earlier (see Chapter 2) topic talk had been<br />

demonstrated to be one <strong>of</strong> the salient ways in which German and English<br />

conversational style differed, in terms <strong>of</strong> both the general orientation to topic<br />

development, and the positive social values oriented to and images <strong>of</strong> selfhood<br />

126

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!