02.03.2013 Views

Downloadable - About University

Downloadable - About University

Downloadable - About University

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

360 Decision framing and cognitive inertia<br />

‘this development would be a technical impossibility’ often go unchallenged.<br />

As we saw earlier, the US automobile industry assumed that<br />

the changeover time between car models could not be reduced below<br />

6–8 hours and they framed their decision accordingly. Similarly, many<br />

of Thomas Edison’s inventions were thought to be infeasible by the<br />

scientific establishment of the day.<br />

False assumptions can also rule out the consideration of courses of<br />

action which, in retrospect, would have been highly desirable. After the<br />

Polaroid camera was invented, Kodak assumed that customers who had<br />

purchased their films and cameras in the past would still be prepared to<br />

wait to have their photographs developed. They ruled out the option of<br />

moving into instant photography until it was too late and lost billions<br />

of dollars as a result. A similar mistake was made by manufacturers of<br />

Swiss watches who assumed that their market for mechanical watches<br />

was unassailable when Japanese companies started making electronic<br />

quartz watches in the late 1960s. They did not respond to the challenge<br />

and sustained huge losses in market share and jobs.<br />

Narrow bracketing of decisions<br />

The way we mentally bracket groups of decisions together often influences<br />

how much risk we are prepared to take when making each<br />

decision. In companies, when projects are evaluated one at a time,<br />

rather than part of an overall portfolio there is likely to be an extreme<br />

unwillingness to take risks. Taking a broader view by looking at<br />

all of the projects together can act as an antidote to this excessive<br />

risk aversion.<br />

For example, Richard Thaler, 5 a US professor, was teaching a group of<br />

executives from one firm, each of whom was responsible for managing a<br />

separate division. He asked each whether he or she would be willing to<br />

undertake a project for the division if it has a 50% chance of generating a<br />

return of $2 million and a 50% chance of losing $1 million. Only three of<br />

the 25 executives accepted the gamble. However, when the company’s<br />

CEO was asked whether he would like to undertake a portfolio of 25 of<br />

these investments he nodded enthusiastically. From the CEO’s perspective,<br />

losses on some of the projects would be more than compensated by<br />

the gains on others.<br />

In the strategic decision-making literature, frame-blindness is closely<br />

linked to what has been termed ‘cognitive inertia’. Consideration of

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!