02.03.2013 Views

Downloadable - About University

Downloadable - About University

Downloadable - About University

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Aggregating judgments in general 313<br />

example, suppose that three individuals, Allen, Bailey and Crossman,<br />

make the following estimates of the cost of launching a new product:<br />

$5 million, $2.5 million and $3 million. We think that Allen is the best<br />

judge and Crossman the worst, and we therefore decide to attach weights<br />

of 0.6, 0.3 and 0.1 to their estimates (note that if the weights do not sum<br />

to one then this can be achieved by normalizing them – see Chapter 3).<br />

The group estimates will therefore be:<br />

(0.6 × $5m) + (0.3 × $2.5m) + (0.1 × $3m) = $4.05m<br />

Clearly, the main problem of using weighted averages is that the<br />

judgmental skills of the group members need to be assessed in order to<br />

obtain the weights. Methods which have been proposed fall into three<br />

categories: self-rating, rating of each individual by the whole group<br />

(see, for example, De Groot 3 ) and rating based on past performance.<br />

However, there can be difficulties in applying these methods. The<br />

first two approaches compound the individual’s judgmental task by<br />

requiring not only judgments about the problem in hand but also those<br />

about the skill of individual group members. In some circumstances these<br />

problems can be avoided by using weights based on past performance,<br />

but as Lock 4 points out, even here there can be difficulties. The current<br />

judgmental task may not be the same as those in the past. For example,<br />

the quantity which an individual has to judge may be less familiar<br />

than those which have been estimated previously. Furthermore, past<br />

performance may be a poor guide where judges have improved their<br />

performance through learning.<br />

Clearly, simple averaging avoids all these problems, so is it worth<br />

going to the trouble of assessing weights? Research in this area has<br />

consistently indicated that simple averages produce estimates which are<br />

either as good as, or only slightly inferior to, weighted averages (see, for<br />

example, Ashton and Ashton 2 ). Ferrell 1 suggests a number of reasons for<br />

this. He argues that groups tend to be made up of individuals who have<br />

very similar levels of expertise and access to the same information. In<br />

the case of small groups, even if we are fortunate enough to identify the<br />

best individual estimate, its accuracy is unlikely to be much better than<br />

that of the simple average of the entire group’s judgments. Ferrell also<br />

points out that few experiments provide the conditions where weighting<br />

would be likely to offer advantages. He suggests that these conditions<br />

exist when there is:<br />

a moderately large group of well-acquainted individuals that frequently<br />

works together and has a wide range of different types of expertise to bring<br />

to bear on questions that require an equally wide range of knowledge.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!