02.03.2013 Views

Downloadable - About University

Downloadable - About University

Downloadable - About University

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

256 Biases in probability assessment<br />

3. Expecting chance to be self-correcting<br />

If a fair coin is tossed and a long sequence of heads appears, many<br />

people will think that the occurrence of a tail on the next throw is highly<br />

probable because the tail is overdue. In a random process, after all, one<br />

would expect the occurrences of heads and tails to be equally frequent.<br />

The phenomenon can also be seen in newspaper articles about lotteries<br />

which advise readers to identify cold numbers, that is, numbers which<br />

have not been drawn for a long period and therefore must have a higher<br />

probability of appearing in the next draw. Of course, coins and lotteries<br />

have no memories so there is no reason at all why they should correct<br />

for earlier sequences of results. This bias is another consequence of the<br />

belief that random sequences of events should be representative of what<br />

a random process is perceived to look like.<br />

Test your judgment: answers to questions 6 and 7<br />

Q6. This thinking is false. At the start of his driving career there may<br />

have been a high probability of the driver having an accident at<br />

some time during the 45-year period. But given that 40 of these<br />

years are now in the past, there is no reason why an accident should<br />

become more likely in order to ‘correct’ for the 40 accident-free<br />

years. You may argue that the driver’s chances of an accident have<br />

increased because 40 accident-free years have bred complacency, or<br />

because he is losing his faculties as he grows older, but this was not<br />

the basis for the argument given in the question.<br />

Q7. No.<br />

4. Ignoring regression to the mean<br />

In the nineteenth century the British scientist Sir Francis Galton found<br />

that relatively tall fathers tended to have sons who were shorter than<br />

them. Short fathers, on the other hand, had sons taller than them. In<br />

both cases the sons tended to be closer than their fathers to the mean<br />

height of adult males. Galton referred to this phenomenon, whereby<br />

events which are above or below average tend to be followed by events<br />

closer to average, as regression to the mean. The phenomenon can be<br />

widely observed in many situations. If you give somebody an intelligence<br />

test and they perform exceptionally well, it is likely that they<br />

will perform less well if you retest them. A month of exceptionally

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!