02.03.2013 Views

Downloadable - About University

Downloadable - About University

Downloadable - About University

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Assessment of decision structure 157<br />

Keeney 15 has fewer reservations:<br />

Often the complex problems are so involved that their structure is not well<br />

understood. A simple decision tree emphasizing the problem structure which<br />

illustrates the main alternatives, uncertainties, and consequences, can usually<br />

be drawn up in a day. Not only does this often help in defining the problem,<br />

but it promotes client and colleague confidence that perhaps decision analysis<br />

can help. It has often been my experience that sketching out a simple decision<br />

tree with a client in an hour can lead to big advances in the eventual solution<br />

to a problem.<br />

Many decision makers report that they feel the process of problem<br />

representation is perhaps more important than the subsequent computations.<br />

Humphreys 16 has labeled the latter the ‘direct value’ of decision<br />

analysis and the former the ‘indirect value’. Decision analysis provides<br />

the decision maker with a<br />

convincing rationale for choice, improves communication and permits direct<br />

and separate comparisons of different people’s conceptions of the structure<br />

of the problem, and of the assessment of decomposed elements within their<br />

structures, thereby raising consciousness about the root of any conflict.<br />

However, some studies have illustrated that the decision makers’<br />

estimates, judgment and choices are affected by the way knowledge<br />

is elicited.<br />

This research has direct relevance for the decision analyst’s attempts<br />

at structuring. In one study, Fischhoff et al. 17 investigated estimation<br />

of failure probabilities in decision problem representations called fault<br />

trees. These fault trees are essentially similar to decision trees, with<br />

the exception that events rather than acts and events are represented.<br />

Figure 6.10 gives a fault tree representation for the event ‘a car fails to<br />

start’. This is the full version of the fault tree that Fischhoff produced<br />

from the use of several car-repair reference texts.<br />

In several experiments Fischhoff presented various ‘full’ and ‘pruned’<br />

fault trees to members of the public. For example, three of the first six<br />

sub-events in Figure 6.10 would be omitted from the presentation to be<br />

implicitly included under the seventh sub-event ‘all other problems’.<br />

Fischhoff asked:<br />

For every 100 times that a trip is delayed due to a ‘starting failure’ estimate,<br />

on average, how many of the delays are caused by the 7(4) factors?<br />

Fischhoff found that the amount of probability placed on ‘all other<br />

problems’ did not increase significantly when it contained three of the

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!