Unser Haushund: Eine Spitzmaus im Wolfspelz? - Wolf-Ekkehard ...

Unser Haushund: Eine Spitzmaus im Wolfspelz? - Wolf-Ekkehard ... Unser Haushund: Eine Spitzmaus im Wolfspelz? - Wolf-Ekkehard ...

13.07.2015 Aufrufe

106law of homologous series in the inheritance of variability (1935) 178 .Wir lesen in der Wikipedia (wieder als Beispiel einer vom generellen Publikumhäufig zu Rate gezogenen Quelle aus der Sekundärliteratur) 179 :"There are many records of domesticated red foxes [see photo below, right] and others, but rarely of sustaineddomestication. A recent and notable case is the Russian silver fox [photo left], or domesticated silver fox, since itresulted in visible and behavioral changes, and is a case study of an animal population modeling according to humandomestication needs. The current group of domesticated silver foxes are the result of nearly fifty years 180 ofexperiments in the Soviet Union and Russia to domesticate the silver morph of the red fox. Notably, the new foxesbecame more tame, allowing themselves to be petted, whimpering to get attention and sniffing and lickingtheir caretakers.[17] They also became more dog-like as well: they lost their distinctive musky "fox smell",became more friendly with humans, put their ears down (like dogs), wagged their tails when happy and began tovocalize and bark like domesticated dogs. They also began to exhibit other traits seen in some dog breeds, such ascolor pattern, curled tails, floppy ears, and shorter legs and tails.[17] They are also more likely to have piebaldcoats, and will almost always have a white spot on the chest or face. The breeding project was set up by the Sovietscientist Dmitri K. Belyaev."Links: Silberfuchs aus: http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Domesticated_silver_foxRechts: "Tame red fox with man in Kent” by Duncan Hall. Aus: http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Red_fox#Taming_and_domestication.Zugriff (beide) 12. September 2012.So sehr ich diese rekurrente und homologe Parallelvariation als weitereBestätigung der oben genannten Gesetze auch schätze – so möchte ich doch imFolgenden sowohl auf die Stärken als auch auf die Schwächen dieser ohne Zweifelbeeindruckenden Parallelvariation zu sprechen kommen, und beim ThemaMakroevolution (Aufbau komplexer neuer Information) nicht zuletzt auch wiederan den Abbau von Funktionen und Strukturen (Degeneration) erinnern, die auchdieses Beispiel völlig unbrauchbar als Beweis für die Makroevolution machen 181 .Dawkins behauptet hingegen (2009/2010, p. 73):"We can get an idea of how tameness, or anything else, can be sculpted – naturally or artificially – by looking at afascinating experiment of modern times, on the domestication of Russian silver foxes for use in the fur trade. It is178 Siehe dazu z. B.: http://www.weloennig.de/VavilovLawofVariation.pdf. Vavilov: "In Pangenesis and Mutationstheorie, we find many factssignifying the existence of parallel variation. "Suchen wir in irgendeiner Flora," writes de Vries in Mutationstheorie, p. 454, "diese abgeleitetenVarietäten zusammen, so fällt sofort auf, dass dieselbe Abweichung in der verschiedensten Familien, Gattungen, und Arten wiederkehrt. Überallbilden die Varietaten Reihen von parallelen Formen." Mutationstheorie, I. p. 454.” Oder noch einmal Vavilov: "So far as we know, this kind ofvariation is not "occasional," as Darwin supposed it to be, but quite general…the detailed study of hundreds of Linnean species belonging todifferent families shows that there are no plants which are an exception to this rule. Therefore, we may conclude that, in general, closely alliedLinnean species are characterized by similar and parallel series of varieties…” (Italics by Vavilov 1922, pp. 57/58).179http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Fox#Domestication (12. September 2012)180 Inzwischen mehr als 50 Jahre, das Experiment begann, wie gesagt nach Trut 1959.181 Für die es seitens der Experimentatoren, zumindest anfangs, ohnehin nicht gedacht war. Vgl. http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Domesticated_silver_fox:"The experiment was initiated by scientists who were interested in the topic of domestication and the process by which wolves becametame domesticated dogs.” Andererseits folgen die Autoren dem Trend des Zeitgeistes, wenn sie undifferenziert von Evolution sprechen, z. B.:Trut, Oskina, and Kharlamova (2009): Animal evolution during domestication: the domesticated fox as a model. Bioessays 31: 349-360.

107doubly interesting because of the lessons it teaches us, over and above what Darwin knew, about the domesticationprocess, about the "side effects” of selective breeding, and about the resemblance, which Darwin well understood,between artificial and natural selection.”Dawkins versucht mit diesem Beispiel unter anderem, den Unterschied zwischenkünstlicher und natürlicher Selektion zu verwischen. Zunächst seine allgemeine imGroßen und Ganzen zutreffende Beschreibung des Versuchsaufbaus:"Belyaev and his colleagues (and successors, for the experimental program continued after his death) subjected foxcubs to standardized tests in which an experimenter would offer a cub food by hand, while trying to stroke or fondle it.The cubs were classified into three classes. Class III cubs were those that fled from or [tried to] bit the person. Class IIcubs would allow themselves to be handled, but showed no positive responsiveness to the experimenters. Class I cubs,the tamest of all, positively approached the handlers, wagging their tails and whining. When the cubs grew up, theexperimenters systematically bred only from this tamest class.”Man beachte nun bei seinen folgenden Erklärungen zum Fuchs-Experiment, wie erimplizit und später explizit damit die Makroevolution veranschaulichen will (p. 74):"After a mere six generations of this selective breeding for tameness, the foxes had changed so much that theexperimenters felt obliged to name a new category, the "domesticated elite” class, which were "eager to establishhuman contact, whimpering to attract attention and sniffing and licking experimenters like dogs”. At the beginning ofthe experiment, none of the foxes were in the elite class."Trut et al. (2012, pp. 14/15) relativieren diese Aussage von Dawkins vielleichtinsofern etwas, als sie feststellen, dass schon 10% der Individuen der wildenAusgangspopulation 182 eine sehr geringe Aggressivität zeigten: "A geneticallydetermined polymorphism for the expression of aggressive and fear responses tohumans was revealed in the farmed fox populations. […] Some of the farm foxesmanifested aggressive responses very weakly. About 10% of the farm-foxes weresuch individuals (Fig. 2.2)." 183 Die Abbildung des Rotfuchses oben (mit dem völligrichtigen Hinweis "There are many records of domesticated red foxes and others,but rarely of sustained domestication.)” zeigt, dass bei einzelnen Exemplaren dieVorstufen zur domesticated elite class möglicherweise übersprungen werdenkönnen. Trut et al. schreiben jedoch (2012, pp. 15/16): "It is noteworthy that tamefoxes from the 'elite of domestication', as well as domestic dogs, are able to readhuman social signals (point and gaze cues) and react adequately to them (Hare etal., 2005). The early behavioural elites appeared at the sixth generation selectedfor tameness. Elite in this context means 'impeccable', or tamed to the highestdegree.” 184 Es wäre vielleicht nicht uninteressant zu erfahren, inwieweit der obenabgebildete Rotfuchs in den Armen seines "Herrchens" und zumindest einige dervielen weiteren Beispiele gezähmter Füchse das nicht vielleicht auch schonkonnten. Aber lassen wir diese Frage offen.In nur sechs Generationen kann man jedenfalls nicht mit allzu vielen neuenMutationen rechnen (und Inzuchteffekte schließen die Autoren aus). Bleibt also zunächst nur dervon Trut al al. genannte "genetically determined polymorphism for the expression182 Trut et al. erklären zu ihrer Ausgangspopulation der Farmfüchse (p. 14): "When the domestication experiment was started, the silver fox hadbeen bred in fur farms for more than 50 years. It may be thought that it had overcome the barrier of natural selection during its alienation fromnature, and its caging and breeding in activity, but it had retained the standard phenotype, strict seasonality of biological functions and therelatively wild behaviour (Fig. 2.1).” – Worauf der oben schon zitierte Hinweis folgt: "A genetically determined polymorphism for the expressionof aggressive and fear responses to humans was revealed in the farmed fox populations. There might have been, quite plausibly, suchpolymorphism in the type of defensive responses to humans in the initial natural populations of wolves.”183 Diese Abbildung zeigt einen Silberfuchs im Käfig mit einer zum Kontakt ausgestreckten Hand (Untertitel "This fox shows a weak aggressiveresponse to attempts to touch it").184 Siehe auch Truts Beschreibung von 1999 unterhttp://www.hum.utah.edu/~bbenham/2510%20Spring%2009/Behavior%20Genetics/Farm-Fox%20Experiment.pdf

107doubly interesting because of the lessons it teaches us, over and above what Darwin knew, about the domesticationprocess, about the "side effects” of selective breeding, and about the resemblance, which Darwin well understood,between artificial and natural selection.”Dawkins versucht mit diesem Beispiel unter anderem, den Unterschied zwischenkünstlicher und natürlicher Selektion zu verwischen. Zunächst seine allgemeine <strong>im</strong>Großen und Ganzen zutreffende Beschreibung des Versuchsaufbaus:"Belyaev and his colleagues (and successors, for the exper<strong>im</strong>ental program continued after his death) subjected foxcubs to standardized tests in which an exper<strong>im</strong>enter would offer a cub food by hand, while trying to stroke or fondle it.The cubs were classified into three classes. Class III cubs were those that fled from or [tried to] bit the person. Class IIcubs would allow themselves to be handled, but showed no positive responsiveness to the exper<strong>im</strong>enters. Class I cubs,the tamest of all, positively approached the handlers, wagging their tails and whining. When the cubs grew up, theexper<strong>im</strong>enters systematically bred only from this tamest class.”Man beachte nun bei seinen folgenden Erklärungen zum Fuchs-Exper<strong>im</strong>ent, wie er<strong>im</strong>plizit und später explizit damit die Makroevolution veranschaulichen will (p. 74):"After a mere six generations of this selective breeding for tameness, the foxes had changed so much that theexper<strong>im</strong>enters felt obliged to name a new category, the "domesticated elite” class, which were "eager to establishhuman contact, wh<strong>im</strong>pering to attract attention and sniffing and licking exper<strong>im</strong>enters like dogs”. At the beginning ofthe exper<strong>im</strong>ent, none of the foxes were in the elite class."Trut et al. (2012, pp. 14/15) relativieren diese Aussage von Dawkins vielleichtinsofern etwas, als sie feststellen, dass schon 10% der Individuen der wildenAusgangspopulation 182 eine sehr geringe Aggressivität zeigten: "A geneticallydetermined polymorphism for the expression of aggressive and fear responses tohumans was revealed in the farmed fox populations. […] Some of the farm foxesmanifested aggressive responses very weakly. About 10% of the farm-foxes weresuch individuals (Fig. 2.2)." 183 Die Abbildung des Rotfuchses oben (mit dem völligrichtigen Hinweis "There are many records of domesticated red foxes and others,but rarely of sustained domestication.)” zeigt, dass bei einzelnen Exemplaren dieVorstufen zur domesticated elite class möglicherweise übersprungen werdenkönnen. Trut et al. schreiben jedoch (2012, pp. 15/16): "It is noteworthy that tamefoxes from the 'elite of domestication', as well as domestic dogs, are able to readhuman social signals (point and gaze cues) and react adequately to them (Hare etal., 2005). The early behavioural elites appeared at the sixth generation selectedfor tameness. Elite in this context means '<strong>im</strong>peccable', or tamed to the highestdegree.” 184 Es wäre vielleicht nicht uninteressant zu erfahren, inwieweit der obenabgebildete Rotfuchs in den Armen seines "Herrchens" und zumindest einige dervielen weiteren Beispiele gezähmter Füchse das nicht vielleicht auch schonkonnten. Aber lassen wir diese Frage offen.In nur sechs Generationen kann man jedenfalls nicht mit allzu vielen neuenMutationen rechnen (und Inzuchteffekte schließen die Autoren aus). Bleibt also zunächst nur dervon Trut al al. genannte "genetically determined polymorphism for the expression182 Trut et al. erklären zu ihrer Ausgangspopulation der Farmfüchse (p. 14): "When the domestication exper<strong>im</strong>ent was started, the silver fox hadbeen bred in fur farms for more than 50 years. It may be thought that it had overcome the barrier of natural selection during its alienation fromnature, and its caging and breeding in activity, but it had retained the standard phenotype, strict seasonality of biological functions and therelatively wild behaviour (Fig. 2.1).” – Worauf der oben schon zitierte Hinweis folgt: "A genetically determined polymorphism for the expressionof aggressive and fear responses to humans was revealed in the farmed fox populations. There might have been, quite plausibly, suchpolymorphism in the type of defensive responses to humans in the initial natural populations of wolves.”183 Diese Abbildung zeigt einen Silberfuchs <strong>im</strong> Käfig mit einer zum Kontakt ausgestreckten Hand (Untertitel "This fox shows a weak aggressiveresponse to attempts to touch it").184 Siehe auch Truts Beschreibung von 1999 unterhttp://www.hum.utah.edu/~bbenham/2510%20Spring%2009/Behavior%20Genetics/Farm-Fox%20Exper<strong>im</strong>ent.pdf

Hurra! Ihre Datei wurde hochgeladen und ist bereit für die Veröffentlichung.

Erfolgreich gespeichert!

Leider ist etwas schief gelaufen!